For each animal, we recorded 3 consecutive training sessions and concatenated those datasets to get the finals with 51 trials from monkey M and 86 trials from monkey M. All trials for analysis were filtered (Methods). For the ‘NORMAL’ task, we aligned the data to the onset of touch and truncated the data from -2 second to 3 second marks to get 5 seconds long segments. For the ‘SURPRISE EVENT’ task, we aligned the data to the onset of the compliance change and truncated the data from -2 second to 3 second marks. Out of the total of 9216 possible LFP channel pairs, we selected 1310 pairs in monkey M and 898 pairs in monkey C which showed significant causality across the multiple stages of Fig. 1B. We chose a target α = 0.05 and used FDR to correct for multiple testing (Methods). We only claimed statistically significant values for a target α = 0.05, with FDR correction for multiple testing. The Causality index GC was evaluated for each direction of cortical communication, i.e., computed GC separately for S1->M1 and M1->S1, respectively.
Beta Oscillations in S1 and M1 during the ‘NORMAL’ task. Both monkeys performed the trained task in a stereotypical sequence: Following initial movement planning and subsequent arm reach, the animals initially overshot the set target force after grasping on the manipulandum. A transient peak in the internal pressure of the manipulandum occurred within 0.2-0.4s after the onset of touch (shown schematically as red solid line Fig. 1B, actual measurement for each monkey shown in upper trace of Fig. 2A). The monkeys then reduced the force of their squeeze to attain the correct pressure mark within approximately 1s. As per the instructed task, they continued to hold and maintain the correct and constant force for several seconds in steady state.
Upon touching the manipulandum, a relatively slow transient modulation in the LFP amplitude and spectrum was recorded from both electrode arrays (Fig. 2A, B). Following the decay of these transients, well-defined oscillations commenced in each area, remaining pronounced through the steady-state, constant squeeze stage. In analyzing the multichannel data from pairs of MEA channels (one channel from S1, another from M1), we tracked the modulation of the LFPs in both amplitude (bootstrap resampling, the number of resamples is 1000) and frequency across each particular stage of the task (‘Planning’, ‘Moving’, ‘Touching’, and ‘Steady-state’, each defined by a colored rectangle in Fig. 1B). For the movement ‘Planning’ stage, we observed that LFP powers remained quite stable in most M1, S1 channels (160/192 channels in monkey M and 123/161 channels in monkey C). However, for the ‘Moving’ and ‘Touching’ stages, we documented a significant decrease in the amplitude of the LFP signals across all channels once the monkeys initiated the arm movement and opened the aperture of the hand to grasp the object. This down-modulation began some two hundred milliseconds before the actual onset of touch (Fig. 2A) with M1 appearing to precede that of S1 by a several milliseconds. Both modulations shared a similar evoked potential waveform, namely a bipolar waveform led by faster negative transient followed by slower positive repolarization. The LFP amplitudes then re-emerged within approximately 0.5 seconds after the onset of touch. We also observed similar downmodulation in the M1 and S1 LFP signals when the monkeys retrieved the hand upon completing a trial.
The dominant oscillatory features recorded from both S1 and M1 frequency matched the range of known beta-range oscillations (15-30Hz, Fig. 2B). The beta oscillations were first evident in both S1 and M1 during the movement ‘Planning’ stage (-2 to -1s). That beta oscillations decrease upon initiation of arm kinematics is of course well-known – here we see beta being replaced by the emergence of a transient low-frequency oscillation. These putative ERPs remained the dominant feature from movement initiation to approximately 1 second after onset of touch, i.e., while the monkeys applied too much force. Upon entry to the steady state (flat region in red solid line of Fig. 1B), the beta- oscillations re-emerged and attained dominance after t> 0 sec, remaining well-defined to the end of the task. Quantitatively, the beta-band power in M1 was larger than in S1 during the steady-state squeeze in monkey M (40 \(\frac{{\mu V}^{2}}{Hz}\) vs 33 \(\frac{{\mu V}^{2}}{Hz}\), random permutation test, p-value = 5*10−3 ) as well as monkey C (25 \(\frac{{\mu V}^{2}}{Hz}\) vs 19 \(\frac{{\mu V}^{2}}{Hz}\), random permutation test, p-value = 10−2, Supplementary Fig. 1A). Figure 2C shows the coherence profile of S1 and M1 through the task. The analysis shows good consistency between the LFP spectrograms and computed coherence.
Granger Causality shows bi-directional communication between the two areas. Performing a nonparametric Granger causality test for selected single S1-M1 LFP pairs, we found that the GC index is bidirectionally modulated across the temporal pressure profile of the manipulandum (proportional to the force of the monkey’s squeeze). Figure 3A displays an example of spectro-temporal Granger causality as heat maps for the two animals showing the presence of bidirectional communication between S1 and M1 across the range of beta-band oscillations, approximately 13-16Hz in Monkey M and 23-30Hz in Monkey C. Age differences have been implicated in variation of the beta band frequency and power in healthy humans 24,25. Given that one of our monkeys was old and the other just reaching adulthood, we suggest that the age factor can be a contributor to the differing beta frequencies.
In both animals, there were only modest S1-M1 detected interactions present during initial resting and movement planning stages (-2 to -1 second). These interactions decreased further while the monkey executed the movement of his arm and hand (-1 to 0 second) and remained low during the ‘touching’ stage (0 to 1 second). However, once a grasp was established, the GC index increased bidirectionally during the progression toward the steady-state. Consistent with the computed coherence, the GC values reached the maximum during steady-state in both monkeys. Figure 3B shows the time course of beta-band averaged GC during the steady-state and its directional dependence (red trace for “M1->S1” in Fig. 3B ;“blue trace for S1->M1” in Fig. 3B).
For a more comprehensive analysis of data in the ‘NORMAL’ task, we evaluated the average beta-band causality index across S1 and M1 channels pairs (1310 selected pairs in monkey M and 898 pairs in monkey C). Viewed across the entire time course of the trials, the causality analysis shows firstly that the statistically evaluated GC index was significant during the initial resting/planning stages when the monkey was resting his hand. The GC index then decreased slightly during the movement kinematics (arm and hand reach). Causality remained low and statistically insignificant during the ‘Touching’ stage. A robust bidirectional increase then followed reaching maximum once the animals entered the steady-state (Fig. 4A), consistent with the example result above for a single individual LFP channel pair.
For evaluation of the distribution of the GC index across S1 and M1 channel pairs, we selected specific timepoints at midpoint of each of the four stages, at -1.5, -0.5, 0.5, 1.5 second marks, respectively. We implemented a kernel density estimation to compute the distribution of GC over selected pairs. Figure 4B shows the distribution of the GC index expressed as violin plots across different task stages. The GC values during steady-state squeeze were significantly larger than for other stages (two-sample t-test, p =0.001). The violin plots lend support the hypothesis of dynamical modulation in the bidirectional S1↔M1 communication during the squeeze of the compliant manipulandum.
To investigate if one cortical area was the principal driver in the inter-area relationship, we compared values of the GC index for S1-->M1 and M1->S1, respectively, by selecting 10 channel pairs which had the GC index closest to the mean of the distribution and performed a random permutation test (1000 permutations) at each stage of the task for each pair (Supplementary Fig. 2A). We found that in the planning stage, both S1->M1 and M1->S1 communication is significant, sharing the same strength. During the movement stage, both communication strength decreased in value but continued to show bidirectionality (in causality). Entering the “touching” stage we find that in monkey M in particular, the GC index for M1->S1 becomes larger. This particular direction of inter-areal communication (M1->S1) further increases and takes on a dominant role once in the steady state (p = 0.001, random permutation test). However, in monkey C, we found results which were more ambiguous. Here, majority 7 of 10 channel pairs show that index for S1-->M1 is dominant in the steady-state while minority 2 of 10 pairs show that the GC indices are comparable, and only 1 channel pair supports M1->S1’s dominance (Supplementary Fig. 2B). This ambiguity between the animals will be discussed further in the Discussion section.
An abrupt, random change in manipulandum compliance modulates LFPs in both amplitude and frequency. Next, we explored cortical communication during the ‘SURPRISE EVENT’, i.e. the impact from introduction of a sudden change in the compliance of the manipulandum. While the monkey was squeezing the manipulandum in steady state, the pressure in the manipulandum was increased at random time points across multiple trials by approximately 4 kPa, the reduction in compliance occurring within ~200ms, (dashed line in Fig. 1B and top trace of Fig. 5A). To illustrate, the middle traces in Fig. 5A show single trial LFP traces for both S1 and M1 where the time origin, t=0, now designates the onset of the abrupt compliance decrease.
Trial-averaged LFP (lowest trace in Fig. 5A) shows that in both S1 and M1 ERPs near t=0, similar to the modulation seen at the onset of touch in NORMAL task (Fig. 2A). In both monkeys we documented a negative discharge of M1 signals followed by a slow recovery. By contrast, we found a strong and fast positive polarization for S1, followed by a slower depolarization. Broadly, these evoked-potentials occurred around 40-80ms following the start of the abrupt compliance change at t=0, lasting approximately 300-600ms.
Analysis of the LFP spectrograms showed likewise the presence of significant evoked modulation at the onset of the imposed compliance change (Fig. 5B). We observed a near complete replacement of beta oscillations by a low-frequency, large-amplitude ERP (bootstrap resampling, the number of resamples is 1000). This spectral modulation occurred around 40-100ms after t=0 (the onset of the ‘SURPRISE EVENT’) and lasted up to 1 second. Thereafter, the ERP power density decreased and was replaced by the re-emergence of the robust beta-band signals once the monkey had found a new stable steady-squeeze state point. We labelled this latter stage as the “Recovery” process. The beta oscillation peak frequencies were consistent with the ones in the first steady-state (Supplementary Fig. 1B).
Single pair coherence and average coherence plotted as heat maps give insight to the dynamics of LFP interarea coherence. We found statistically significant levels of LFP coherence in the beta range prior to the onset of the SURPRISE EVENT (from -2 to 0 second), consistent with the result from the ‘NORMAL task. At t=0, the beta coherence ramped down with the disappearance of beta frequency (spectrograms of Fig. 5C for S1 and M1). In both monkeys, this reduction in coherence lasted approximately 1 second, where after the beta-band coherence rallied, as the animal’s hand squeeze reached a new steady-state.
Abrupt and randomly timed change in compliance interrupts bidirectional S1-M1 LFP communication, which recovers when the animal finds a new steady-state. We again resorted to the Granger Causality in examining the impact of the compliance perturbation on S1↔M1 communication. We computed the GC index while focusing specifically on a 5 second interval around the onset of the SURPRISE EVENT. We first computed the GC for a selected single LFP channel pair to find what effect, if any, did our recordings unveil. Figure 6A shows the heat map of the GC index for this S1-M1 pair. During the initial steady squeeze preceding the compliance change, i.e. the interval from -2 to 0 seconds, the GC values for both S1->M1 and M1->S1 directions showed expected levels of causation as in the NORMAL task. Following the surprise event at t=0, the GC index in both directions decreased significantly (Fig. 6B). However, once the animal had re-calibrated his hand squeeze (force), typically in 1 second, a new steady-state was established and the GC index recovered.
For statistical evaluation across multiple microelectrodes, we computed averaged GC values across many LFP S1-M1 channel pairs (1310 selected pairs in monkey M and 898 pairs in monkey C). We found overall similarity to the case of a single channel pair above. Again, the averaged GC values were statistically significant during the steady-state preceding the ‘SURPRISE EVENT’ as found in the ‘NORMAL’ task (Fig. 7A). As the compliance of the manipulandum was changed, a pronounced decrease in the statistically averaged GC indices for both S1->M1 and M1->S1 directions occurred. The GC index reached a minimum at approximately 0.4 - 0.5s, consistent with the observed LFP envelope modulation in Fig. 5. We suggest that this loss of causality indicates partial interruption in the inter-areal communication, mirrored in the beta band activity, allowing the involved cortical areas to adapt to the unexpected perturbation for the network find a new steady state. The GC index recovered quite quickly to reach a maximum in approximately 1-1.5 seconds in monkey M and 0.5-1 seconds in monkey C.
As a statistical summary, Fig. 7B shows the ‘violin plots’ in the distribution of the GC index across the three-time intervals of interest here: the initial steady-state squeeze, the SURPRISE EVENT, and the Recovery stage leading to a new steady-state of the hand squeeze (prior to hand release). As expected, the reduction in bidirectional communication is reflected in the violin plots. Quantitatively, for Monkey C, the index for S1->M1 is highest in the steady-state period, while M1->S1 showed the highest (relative) values during the Recovery period. For Monkey M, the M1->S1 index appeared to dominate the both Steady-state and Recovery period. (Two-sample t-test, p =10−3 for each).