It should be noted that the EEG power in this study is low. It may be caused by the calculation method of power. In the research using the same power calculation method, the beta frequency band varies in the range of 0.0005–0.007, which is not high either31,32. Therefore, even if the value of Alpha power is low (basically between 0.0013 and 0.0025), we still report them objectively.
The distraction effect arises in large part due to a shift in brain resources. (Wang et al., 2014) We found a consistent trend in drivers' prefrontal, frontal, occipital, and temporal regions, i.e. a gradual decrease in alpha power values during driving phase 2.3 (20-30 minutes) for drivers in the simple talk content group in automatic driving mode. The trend was the opposite for the complex talk content group, with a gradual increase. At 20 minutes of driving, the alpha power values in the prefrontal areas of the simple talk content group were significantly lower than those of the simple talk group in the manual driving group. The prefrontal cortex has been thought to play an important role in cognitive control, i.e. the coordination of thoughts and actions according to internal goals. Cognitive control stems from the active maintenance of prefrontal cortex activity patterns that represent goals and the means to achieve those goals. (Miller et al., 2001) Thus, the results of the EEG data suggest that the mental load induced by the content of a simple mobile phone call is higher in the automatic driving mode for about 10-20 minutes of driving time than in the manual driving state during the same period, occupying more brain resources in the driver's prefrontal area and leading to a reduction in his or her cognitive control. At the same time, the trends in the detection response task performance data during this phase were consistent with the EEG data. The trend in Figure 8 shows that at a driving duration of 10 minutes, the automatic driving simple talk content group had faster response times than the manual driving simple talk content group. When driving for 20 minutes, the autopilot simplex content group's response times slowed rapidly and converged with the manual simple content group. Although the trend in Figure 9 indicates that the autopilot simple content group was more correct during this period compared to the manual group, the EEG data shows that the overall driver load was still high during this period, as their cognitive control was reduced and the excessive attentional resources were disruptive to the driver's reaction time to detect peripheral signals, resulting in progressively slower reaction times.
The results of Hossam Almahasneh's (2014) study found that the most affected brain region during distracted driving was the right frontal cortex. The present study builds on this finding by examining the complexity of drivers' secondary tasks. The most significant difference in the detection of distracted driving was found in the prefrontal regions of the frontal lobe when the complexity of the secondary task was varied, suggesting that mobile phone conversations impair cognitive control in the prefrontal regions of the driver. Meanwhile, Lin et al. (2011) suggested that changes in the alpha band in the frontal region were associated with distracted driving. The EEG results of the present study suggest that a decrease in alpha wave power values in the prefrontal region is a valid indicator for identifying increased mental load in drivers due to distracted mobile phone use. This complements another brain region band indicator for the detection of distracted driving, whereas previous studies have been limited to considering changes in theta wave band as a marker of cognitive distraction in drivers. (Dong et al, 2011; Lin et al., 2009; Victor et al., 2005)
Also, previous studies have found that drivers performing interactive cognitive tasks during prolonged driving appear to improve alertness and driving performance. (Gershon et al., 2009) We additionally found that in the autopilot mode, drivers in the complex mobile phone talk content group had significantly higher alpha wave power values in the occipital region than the manual mode complex talk content group for the same period (30-50 min), and that the complex mobile phone talk content group had significantly higher alpha wave power values in the frontal region than the manual mode complex talk content group for the same period (60 min). Since power changes in frontal regions mainly reflect the degree of activation of intrinsic neurons when individuals allocate their attention to different task stimuli (Missonnier et al., 2006), frontal areas are involved in impulse control, judgement, language production, working memory, motor function, and problem solving. (Burgess et al., 2000) This therefore suggests that activation in the occipital areas of drivers in the complex mobile phone call content group was lower in the automatic driving mode compared to the manual driving state during the same period (30-50 minutes) and that activation in the frontal areas of the complex mobile phone call content group was lower in the automatic driving compared to the manual driving mode during the same period (60 minutes). In contrast, parietal circuits, the prefrontal cortex, and corticolimbic structures were shown to be involved in the distribution of individual directed attention networks together with the medial pulvinar nucleus. (Bakeydier & Mauguiere, 1985) This nucleus projects and receives visual input from the occipital cortex and the superior colliculus, forming an important link with the hippocampus for further memory processing. (Van Hoesen& Pandya, 1975) This suggests that the effects of mobile phone conversations on activation of the occipital cortex as well as the frontal cortex are shared. When drivers engage in complex mobile phone conversations, even when on autopilot, brain functions such as problem solving, judgement and impulse control are impaired as the duration of driving increases. The present study also refines the findings of Saxby (2017) et al. by exploring the complexity of mobile phone calls, and finds that even complex calls, in monotonous prolonged autopilot mode, are not a safe way to reduce fatigue and increase alertness, but instead can be detrimental to driver function in various brain regions.
We also found consistent trends through changes in EEG, pupil diameter, and vigilance detection response task data. The EEG data results showed that the alpha power values in the prefrontal areas of the complex talk content group were significantly lower than those of the simple talk content group at 10 minutes of driving time in automatic driving mode. For 20-30 minutes of driving time in manual driving mode, the alpha power values in the prefrontal areas of the complex talk content group were significantly lower than those of the simple talk content group. However, the difference decreased with increasing driving time, and no significant difference was observed. At 60 minutes of driving time, the prefrontal and temporal data showed a consistent trend in mental load levels regardless of the driving mode and level of complexity.
The trend in pupil diameter indicates that both simple and complex calls in manual driving mode directly lead to an increase in the driver's psychological load, with complex calls inducing a faster and deeper increase in psychological load. In the initial phase of driving (10-20 minutes), the pupil diameter of the drivers in the simple talk content group was significantly smaller in the automatic driving group than in the manual driving group. However, as the duration of driving increased, none of the differences between the pupil diameters of the autopilot group and the manual driving group were significant.
The PDT alert detection response task data showed that during the initial phase of driving (around 10 minutes), when the driver was talking on a mobile phone, the PDT detection response task reaction time was significantly slower in the manual driving group than in the automatic driving group, and the correct PDT detection response task rate was significantly lower in the manual driving group than in the automatic driving group. As the driving time increased, the PDT detection reaction time in the automatic driving group gradually became slower and converged with that of the manual driving group. There was a tendency to overtake the manual driving group after 60 minutes.
All three data results above indicate a common trend that regardless of the level of complexity of mobile phone conversations a driver engages in, the level of psychological load tends to converge between manual and automatic driving modes as the length of driving time increases, suggesting that mobile phone conversations in automatic driving modes also disrupt the driver's cognitive resource balance state, leading to reduced cognitive control and impairing driving safety. The trend of the findings also validates the previous view on the cumulative effect of psychological load (Regan et al., 2009) and refines previous findings (Atchely, et al., 2014; Saxby, et al., 2017) by further delineating the impairment of drivers' use of mobile phone calls during different driving phases in the autonomous driving mode.
For the parietal region, alpha power values were higher in the simple talk group than in the complex talk group at stage 6. In the frontal and occipital regions, the alpha power values of the simple talk group were found to be lower than those of the complex talk group at stage 6. A possible explanation for this is that the occipital component is very close to the parietal area and the left and right motor areas of the frontal lobe, and because of the interconnectedness and complexity of the brain areas, brain areas located in the same cortical layer interact with each other while acting separately on the body (Van Hoesen & Pandya, 1975). It is also possible that at the end of the driving phase, EEG signal acquisition is affected by the driver's somatic fluctuations, resulting in variable alpha-wave brain power values during stage 6.
As automated systems move towards higher levels (L4~L5), the frequency and risk of mobile phone use by drivers remains a key safety concern. Future research needs to further explore the ecology and empirical evidence of mobile phone call content; to delineate the impact of mobile phone calls on the conversion of resources in the driver's brain area at other wave frequencies; to distinguish how the cognitive load from mobile phone calls and the psychological load from fatigue cross over to affect the driver in automated driving mode; and how the psychological load on the driver's mobile phone use changes in longer (more than 60 minutes) automated driving situations, as well as how the psychological load of mobile phone use changes in longer (more than 60 minutes) autonomous driving situations.
In summary, this study compares the safety implications of mobile phone distraction in autonomous driving mode with the use of mobile phones in conventional driving situations; provides a rough delineation of when and for how long mobile phone conversations take place; compares the changes in EEG components in various brain regions in the driver's alpha wave band during mobile phone conversations. This provides a theoretical basis for the development of laws and regulations and policy implications for the use of mobile phones in the field of autonomous driving, where autonomous driving systems require drivers to constantly monitor road hazards and be ready to take over the vehicle, and where the overload caused by mobile phone use can compromise driving safety. At the same time, mobile phone conversations are not a consistent and effective response to the problem of underload in autonomous driving, as they can impair the normal functioning of the driver's brain functions, such as cognitive control, problem solving, and judgement, while increasing the psychological load and compromising driving safety.