Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Broadbent, D. E. (1957). A mechanical model for human attention and immediate memory. Psychological review, 64(3), 205-215.
Bruno, N. M., Embon, I., Díaz Rivera, M. N., Giménez, L., D'Amelio, T. A., Torres Batán, S., et al. (2020). Faster might not be better: Pictures may not elicit a stronger unconscious priming effect than words when modulated by semantic similarity. Consciousness and Cognition, 81, 102932.
Chao, H. (2018). The role of awareness in the cognitive control of single-prime negative priming. Consciousness and Cognition, 57, 94-105.
Chen, J., Chen, H., Ma, L., Jiang, Y. (2020). A Review of Studies on Unconscious Emotional Priming. Open Access Library Journal, 7, e6284.
Deutsch, J. A., Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological review, 70(1), 80-90.
Eimer, M. (1999). Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. Acta Psychologica, 101(2-3), 293-313.
Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). The Discovery of the Unconscious: The History and Evolution of Dynamic Psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.
Eriksson, J., Fontan, A. L., Pedale, T. (2020). Make the Unconscious Explicit to Boost the Science of Consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 260.
Faivre, N., Mudrik, L., Schwartz, N., Koch, C. (2014). Multisensory Integration in Complete Unawareness Evidence From Audiovisual Congruency Priming. Psychological science, 25(11), 2006-2016.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical science. Behavior Research methods, 2007, 39 (2), 175-199.
Finkbeiner, M., Forster, K., Nicol, J., Nakamura, K. (2004). The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming. Journal of memory and language, 51(1), 1-22.
Hirschhorn, R., Kahane, O., Gur-Arie, I., Faivre, N., Mudrik, L. (2021). Windows of Integration Hypothesis Revisited. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 617187.
Howard, C. J., Holcombe, A. O. (2010). Unexpected changes in direction of motion attract attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(8), 2087-2095.
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., Fang, F., Huang, M., He, S. (2006). A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45), 17048-17052.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort (Vol. 1063). Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.
Kimchi, R., Devyatko, D., Sabary, S. (2018). Can perceptual grouping unfold in the absence of awareness? Comparing grouping during continuous flash suppression and sandwich masking. Consciousness and Cognition, 60, 37-51.
Lin, J. Y., Murray, S. O., Boynton, G. M. (2009). Capture of attention to threatening stimuli without perceptual awareness. Current Biology, 19(13), 1118-1122.
Liu, C., Sun, Z., Jou, J., Cui, Q., Zhao, G., Qiu, J., et al. (2016). Unconscious Processing of Facial Emotional Valence Relation: Behavioral Evidence of Integration between Subliminally Perceived Stimuli. Plos One, 11(9), e162689.
Montoro, P. R., Luna, D., Ortells, J. J. (2014). Subliminal Gestalt grouping: Evidence of perceptual grouping by proximity and similarity in absence of conscious perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 1-8.
Nakamura, K., Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., Kouider, S. (2007). Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 104(49), 19643-19648.
Ovando-Tellez, M., Rohaut, B., George, N., Bieth, T., Hugueville, L., Ibrahim, Y., et al. (2021). Does adding beer to coffee enhance the activation of drinks? An ERP study of semantic category priming. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1-16.
Palmer, S. E. (2000). Encyclopedia of psychology, Vol. 6. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 93-97). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Pohl, C., Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J. (2010). Early and late selection in unconscious information processing. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 36(2), 268-285.
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 109(2), 160-174.
Schlaghecken, F., Eimer, M. (2000). A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 62(7), 1367-1382.
Shin, K., Stolte, M., Chong, S. C. (2009). The effect of spatial attention on invisible stimuli. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(7), 1507-1513.
Soto, D., Sheikh, U. A., Rosenthal, C. R. (2019). A Novel Framework for Unconscious Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(4), 242-248.
Tu, S., Wan, S., Jou, J., Ma, Y., Zhao, G., Pan, W. (2020). Can unconscious sequential integration of semantic information occur when the prime Chinese characters are displayed from left to right? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(3), 1221-1229.
Van Opstal, F. (2021). The same-different task as a tool to study unconscious processing. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 37, 35-40.
Verleger, R., Jaskowski, P., Aydemir, A., Lubbe, R. H. V. D., Groen, M. (2004). Qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 133(4), 494-514.
Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(10), 6275-6280.
Wang, D., Tang, L., Liang, Q., Jou, J., Ma, Y., Pan, W., et al. (2017). Unconscious integration of pointing relation between two masked arrows. Visual Cognition, 25(9-10), 928-939.