3.1 HZ 5xFAD mice showed severe neurological deficits
We used a battery of neurological procedures to assess cognitive impairment of somatosensory and sensorimotor reflexes in two age ranges in the 5xFAD mouse model. At 4-6 months of age, although not significantly so, slight impairment in somesthesis (H(3,N=17)= 4.934, P > 0.05), corneal (H(3,N=17)= 0.277, P > 0.05), auditory (H(3,N=17)= 0.277, P > 0.05), righting (H(3,N=17)= 1.429, P > 0.05), inclined plane test (H(3,N=17)= 1.429, P > 0.05) and extension reflexes (H(3,N=17)= 3.048, P > 0.05) was observed in HZ mice (Table 1). However, at older age, HZ 5xFAD mice showed high neurological deficits in head shaking (H(3,N=34)= 8.067, P < 0.05) and corneal reflexes (H(3,N=34)= 8.642, P < 0.05), vibrissae (H(3,N=34)= 6.274, P < 0.05), auditory (H(3,N=34)= 7.052, P < 0.05), righting (H(3,N=34)= 10.39, P<0.05) and extension reflexes (H(3,N=34)= 18.30, P < 0.0001) compared to no-tg group (Table 1). Nevertheless, HTZ mice showed preservation of neurological functions at any age.
3.2 HTZ and HZ 5xFAD mice showed a different taste profile to a preference for a palatable sweet solution
Saccharin and sucrose preference tests were used as an indicator of anhedonia, based on the natural preference of rodent for sweet taste. Sucrose preference is also indicative of motivation for caloric foods. Two-way ANOVA showed genotype (F(2,42)= 5.437, P < 0.001) and age (F(1,42)= 7.222, P < 0.01) effects on saccharin preference since saccharin intake was significantly higher in HZ at 10-12 months than that observed in no-tg mice (Tukey’s tests: P < 0.05, Figure 1A). In contrast, a genotype x age interaction was observed (Two-way ANOVA: F(2,41)= 3.591, P < 0.05) in sucrose preference. Post-hoc analysis revealed that both HTZ and HZ at 4-6 and 10-12 months of age significantly decreased sucrose intake compared to non-transgenic mice, suggesting anhedonia (Tukey test: P < 0.05, Figure 1B). Thus, transgenic 5xFAD mice displayed a preference below the anhedonic threshold set at 65% (one-simple test: P < 0.05). These contradictory data on saccharin versus sucrose preference could be related to the different motivation for food (lower food intake and negative energy balance) shown by 5XFAD mice (López-Gambero et al., 2021).
Regarding animal weight during preference tests, two-way ANOVA revealed a genotype effect from 4-6 months old (SacPT: 4-6 months: F(2,72)= 20.08, P < 0.0001; 10-12 months: F(2,100)= 10.15, P < 0.0001; SPT: 4-6 months: F(2,80)= 7.217, P < 0.01; 10-12 months: F(2,100)= 13.62, P < 0.0001; see supplementary Figure S1A and S1B). The HZ group had a significantly lower weight than the non-tg group maintained for 4 days of preference testing (Tukey’s tests: P < 0.05).
3.3 HTZ and HZ 5xFAD mice showed an abnormal emotional response in OF and EPM
OF and EPM tests were conducted to assess emotional status by monitoring anxiety-like behaviours (Malleret et al., 1999). Two-way ANOVA revealed an effect of genotype (F(2,45)= 6.167, P < 0.01) and age (F(1,45)= 5.527, P < 0.05) on time spent in the centre of the OF (Figure 1C). At 10-12 months of age, HZ mice showed less time spent in the centre compared to non-tg mice, and a similar trend was observed at 4-6 months old, although it was not significant. No differences in locomotion were observed between genotypes (Figure 1D).
In the EPM, less time spent in the open arms has been associated with anxiety-like behaviour (Malleret et al., 1999). Surprisingly, both HTZ and HZ groups showed a significant increase in the time spent in the open arms (one-way ANOVA: F(2,29)= 7.490, P<0.01; Tukey’s tests: P < 0.05, Figure 1E) from 10-12 months of age, being present at earlier times in the 4-6 months old HZ mice. These paradoxical results could be related to the sensorial deficits/cognitive impairment exhibited by the HZ mice, which are unable to assess the risks of the exposed arms (narrow arm width, high illumination, and high height). Therefore, abnormal vibrissae perception in 5xFAD could explain this behavioural response, as described previously (Flanigan et al., 2014). Following this rationale, we cannot interpret this increase in time in the exposed arms as anxiolytic behaviour. No differences in locomotion were observed between genotypes (Figure 1F).
3.4 HZ 5xFAD mice showed impaired spatial working memory
We performed a Y-maze test to assess spatial working memory (Hughes, 2004). Statistical analysis showed that the HZ group had impaired working memory at 10-12 months of age (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,43)= 6.801, P < 0.01). Only HZ mice differed from the HTZ and non-transgenic littermates showing a significant reduction in the percentage of SAB (Tukey’s tests: P < 0.05, Figure 1G). No significant differences in locomotion and number of arm entries were observed between genotypes (Figure 1H and supplementary Figure S1C). In addition, there was no correlation between SAB and number of arm entries and locomotion (Pearson’s comparisons: P > 0.05, see supplementary Figure S1D), suggesting that the cognitive impairment observed only in the HZ group was not produced by locomotor or exploratory activities.
(A) Saccharin preference test. (B) Sucrose preference test. Dashed lines represent the criterion for anhedonia ≤ 65%. (C) Time spent in the centre and (D) locomotion in the open filed. (E) Time spent in the open arms and (F) locomotion in the elevated plus maze. (G) Spontaneous alternation behaviour and (H) locomotion in the Y-maze test. All data represent the mean ± SEM. Tukey's Multiple Comparisons Test: difference transgenic vs non-transgenic mice. (*) P < 0.05; (**) P < 0.01, (****) P < 0.0001.
3.5 HZ 5xFAD mice showed impaired cognitive flexibility in the Morris water maze
Spatial reference learning and memory were assessed using the Morris water maze test paradigm at both ages. In habituation training, we found no significant differences between genotype on path length and swimming speed at either age (one-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, see supplementary Figure S2A-B), although 4–6 month old 5xFAD transgenic mice showed a slight decrease in swimming speed (one-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, see supplementary Figure S2C-D).
In the visual learning, surprisingly 4-6 month old HTZ mice showed a longer escape latency compared to HZ and non-tg mice on the first day of training (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,128)= 12.20, P < 0.0001; trial session effect: F(7,128)= 2.664, P < 0.05; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2A), although on the second day of training they learned the platform location as efficiently as possible compared to non-tg mice. Likewise, the 4-6 month old HTZ mice showed a higher cumulative distance to visible escape latency on the first day of training compared to the non-tg group (two-way ANOVA: genotype x trial session interaction: F(2,32)= 4.040, P < 0.05; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, see supplementary Figure S2E). No significant differences were found between genotypes on path length in the peripheral zone (two-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, see Supplementary Figure S2F). However, the 10-12 month old HZ mice showed longer escape latency compared to non-tg mice (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,176)= 39.03, P < 0.0001; trial session effect: F(7,176)= 8.324, P < 0.0001; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2B) and cumulative distance (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,44)= 5.773, P < 0.01; trial session effect: F(1,44)= 5.509, P < 0.05; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05 (see supplementary Figure S2G) compared to non-tg mice. In addition, we found that swimming strategies differed by genotype and age. For example, 10-12 month old HZ mice had a clear ‘wall-hugging’ or thigmotaxis (time spent in the peripheral zone) behaviour during visual learning compared to HTZ and non-tg mice, which showed a direct search pattern to reach the platform (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,46)= 31.40, P < 0.0001; day effect: F(2,46)= 12.80, P < 0.0001; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, see Supplementary Figure S2H). For navigation of all mice, a positive correlation was found between time spent in the peripheral zone and escape latency (r2= 0.76, P < 0.01; i.e., much time in the peripheral zone and much escape latency, see supplementary Figure S2I).
During acquisition spatial learning, all animals successfully learned the position of the hidden escape platform in target quadrant 1 (Q1) at both 4-6 months and 10-12 months of age, although HTZ mice showed a worse learning curve at earlier age that had a longer escape latency (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,64)= 34.43, P < 0.0001, Tukey’s test: P≤ 0.05, Figure 2C) and cumulative distance to reach the hidden platform (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,67)= 27.10, P < 0.0001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2D) than non-tg mice. However, the 10–12-month-old HTZ mice were efficient at reaching the goal, so they did not show noticeable memory impairment. For the HZ condition, 10-12 month-old mice showed increased escape latency and cumulative distance compared to non-tg mice (two-way ANOVA: escape latency: genotype effect: F(2,88)= 13.24, P < 0.0001; trial session effect: F(3,88)= 5.643, P < 0.01; cumulative distance: genotype effect: F(2,88)= 10.80, P < 0.0001; trial session effect: F(3,88)= 4.474, P < 0.001; Turkey test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2E and F). In addition, the 10-12 month-old HZ group continued to show thigmotaxis behaviour (two-way ANOVA; genotype effect: F(2,92)= 5.275, P < 0.001; training day effect: F(3,92)= 2.806, P < 0.05; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, see supplementary Figure S2J).
Interestingly, we observed no significant differences between the 5xFAD transgenic groups and non-tg mice in the first memory retention test at any age (two-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, Figure 2G and H). All mice in two age ranges spent more time in the target Q1 (two-way ANOVA: 4-6 months: quadrant effect: F(3,64)= 36.66, P < 0.0001, 10-12 months: quadrant effect: F(3,88)= 87.70, P < 0.0001, Figure 2G and H). Supplementary Figure S2K shows the path travelled in the memory retention test 1 of the two age ranges. Nevertheless, during reversal spatial learning, 4-6 month-old HZ mice took significantly longer to reach the location of the new platform in Q3 (one-way ANOVA: P > 0.05; Figure 2I), and this difference was more noticeable at 10-12 months of age (one-way ANOVA: F(2,23)= 4.191, P < 0.05; Figure 2J). The second memory retention test was performed 24 hours after reversal training. The 4-6 month-old HZ mice continued to remember the location of the previous platform in Q1, persisting much longer than non-tg mice (two-way ANOVA: 4-6 months: quadrant effect: F(3,64)= 4.786, P < 0.01; 10-12 months: genotype effect: F(2,88)= 6.956, P < 0.01; quadrant effect: F(3,88)= 36.76, P < 0.0001; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2K and L). Supplementary Figure S2L shows the path travelled in memory retention test 1 of the two age ranges. Finally, we further assessed the memory extinction deficit. At 4-6 months, all genotypes showed a memory extinction in which they spent less time in Q1 after 72 hours of the last acquisition learning (two-way ANOVA: time effect: F(2,48)= 5.578, P < 0.01; Figure 2M). However, the impairment of memory extinction was significant at 10-12 months of age. Statistical analysis revealed that HZ mice showed a higher preference for the first goal and perseverative navigation even after 72 hours, indicating a lack of cognitive flexibility in this age range to develop adequate new spatial learning (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,66)= 5.364, P < 0.01, time effect: F (2,66)= 3.662, P < 0.05; Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 2N).
Escape latency in visual learning at 4-6 months (A) and 10-12 months of age (B). Escape latency (C) and cumulative distance (D) in the acquisition spatial learning at 4-6 months of age. Escape latency (E) and the cumulative distance (F) in the acquisition spatial learning at 10-12 months of age. Time spent in the quadrants in the memory retention test at (G) 4-6 months and (H) 10-12 months of age. Escape latency in the reversal spatial learning at 4-6 months (I) and 10-12 months (J) of age. Time spent in the quadrants in the memory retention test at 4-6 months (K) and 10-12 months of age (L).
3.6 Hippocampal dysfunction in HTZ and HZ 5xFAD mice
To determine whether the genetic loading led a differential pattern of Aβ production and deposition, we quantified Aβ40, Aβ42 and total Aβ in the hippocampal area DG, CA1, and CA3 of 5xFAD transgenic mice by immunohistochemistry. In both HTZ and HZ 5xFAD mice, Aβ deposition was significantly visible in the whole hippocampal area from 4-6 months of age (two-way ANOVA: Aβ40: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,44) = 4.268, P < 0.01, Figure 3A left and B; Aβ42: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,44) = 6.577, P < 0.001, Figure 3C left and D; Total Aβ: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,44) = 15.34, P < 0.01, Figure 3E left and F). As predicted, Aβ accumulation was increased in older 5xFAD transgenic mice (two-way ANOVA: Aβ40: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,40) = 19.61, P < 0.001, Figure 3A right and B; Aβ42: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,40) = 12.75, P < 0.001, Figure 3C right and D; Total Aβ: ‘genotype x area’ interaction: F(6,40) = 27.75, P < 0.001, Figure 3E right and F), being more prominent in HZ 5xFAD mice especially in the aggregation of Aβ42 and total Aβ (Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05).
Regarding the neuroinflammatory response, measured as glial (astrocytes and microglia) reactivity, from 4-6 month old HZ mice showed a greater increase in GFAP densitometry in the whole hippocampal area than non-tg and HTZ (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,27) = 45.38, P < 0.001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 3G left and H), having these differences maintained at 10-12 months of age (Figure 3G right and H). At 10-12 months of age, HTZ mice showed an increase in GFAP densitometry (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,33) = 119.2, P < 0.001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 3G right and H). However, from 4-6 months of age, both HTZ and HZ mice showed increased Iba1 densitometry in the whole hippocampal area compared to non-tg (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,27) = 54.47, P < 0.001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 3I left and J). At 10-12 months of age, we observed similar values (two-way ANOVA: genotype effect: F(2,45) = 64.94, P < 0.001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 3I right and J).
Quantification of (A) β-amyloid 1-40 (Aβ40), (C) β-amyloid 1-42 (Aβ42), and (E) total β-amyloid (Aβ) in the hippocampus of the three experimental groups at 4-6 months (left) and 10-12 months of age (right). Images correspond to representative immunostaining of (B) Aβ40, (D) Aβ42, and (F) total of Aβ. Quantification of (G) GFAP and (I) Iba1 expression in the hippocampus of the three groups at 4-6 months (left) and 10-12 months of age (right). Representative immunostaining of (H) GFAP and (J) Iba1 in the hippocampus of each group was performed in the dentate gyrus (DG), CA1, CA3 and whole hippocampus (Total). Histograms represent the mean ± S.E.M. (n=6). Tukey’s Multiple Comparisons Test: difference between transgenic vs non-transgenic mice: (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01, (***) P < 0.001 and (****) P < 0.0001; difference between HTZ and HZ condition: ($) P < 0.05, ($$) P < 0.01, ($$$) P < 0.001 and ($$$$) P < 0.0001.
On the other hand, the IRS1-PI3K-Akt pathway, which is primarily activated by insulin, is an important hub of physiological responses fundamental for healthy aging. To this end, we aimed to evaluate the activation and inhibition of its substrates in the hippocampus of 5xFAD transgenic mice at 4-6 and 10-12 months of age. Figure 4 displays the western blot analysis of the insulin-PI3K-Akt signalling pathway. We first analysed the phosphorylation of IRS1, which is the initial input of the Akt pathway. Serine 612 phosphorylation was enhanced in the HZ group, suggesting a clear inhibition of the IRS1 at 10-12 months of age since phosphorylation at this serine residue is linked to inactivation of IRS1 signalling toward PI3K (De Fea & Roth, 1997) (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,27)= 8.016, P < 0.01; Figure 4A) when compared to the HTZ and no-tg groups. This is most often seen when the insulin receptor is hypo-active because of a lack of insulin activation. This fact is seen only in the HZ older group, being significantly higher compared to the younger group (Tukey test: P ≤ 0.01, Figure 4A). Subsequently, we analysed the active phosphorylation state of PI3K (p-PI3K) and Akt (p-Akt). The activating phosphorylation of Tyrosine 607 of the p85 PI3K’s subunit decreased significantly in both the HTZ and the HZ groups at 4-6 months of age (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,28)= 6.271, P < 0.01; Figure 4B), being re-established in both groups at 10-12 months-old mice (Tukey test: P ≤ 0.05). However, the levels of total PI3K protein remained unchanged (Tukey test: ns, Figure 4B). Showing a similar profile, the activating phosphorylation of Serine 473 of Akt also presented a significant decrease in 4-6-month-old HZ mice (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,28)= 6.457, P < 0.01; Figure 4C) and been also re-established at 10-12 months of age in the HZ (Tukey test: P ≤ 0.01). The total amount of Akt total is simultaneously decreased in 4–6-month-old HZ mice (Tukey test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 4C). According to increased activity of Akt at 10-12 months of age, the phosphorylation of its substrate GSK-3β (p-GSK3β) at Serine 9 was also raised in the HZ group (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,27)= 4.140, P < 0.01; Figure 4D), thus inhibiting the kinase GSK-3β. When compared to the younger group, the phosphorylation state of Serine 2448 of the mTOR protein, a downstream substrate of Akt, was significantly higher in the HZ at 10-12 months of age (two-way ANOVA: age effect: F(2,28)= 4.998, P < 0.05; Figure 4E). By last, PTEN (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,28)= 13.48, P < 0.0001; Figure 4F) and PP2A (two-way ANOVA: interaction: F(2,28)= 15.92, P < 0.0001; Figure 4F), two Akt phosphatases, were also analysed revealing a substantial reduction in total protein at 4-6 months old, with levels entirely reversed at 12 months in both HTZ and HZ groups (Tukey test: P ≤ 0.01).
Bar charts represent the expression of (A) Insulin receptor substrate 1 (IRS-1) phosphorylation at serine 612, and amount of total IRS-1. (B) p85 regulatory domain of the phosphatidylinositol 3 kinase (p85-PI3K) phosphorylation at tyrosine 607, and quantity of total p85-PI3K. (C) Protein Kinase B (Akt) phosphorylation on serine 473, and amount of total Akt. (D) Glycogen synthase kinase 3β (GSK-3β) phosphorylation at serine 9, and amount of total GSK-3β. (E) mammalian Target of Rapamycin (mTOR) phosphorylation at serine 2448, and amount of total mTOR; (F) amount of total PTEN and PP2C. The blots shown are a representation of all bands at (G) 4-6-months of age, and (H) 10-12 months of age (See Supplementary Figures S3 and S4 for additional information). The corresponding expression of γ-Adaptin is shown as a loading control per lane. All samples were obtained at the same time and processed in parallel. Histograms represent the mean ± SEM (n=6). Bonferroni tests were performed: difference between transgenic vs non-transgenic mice: (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01, (***) and P < 0.001; difference between HTZ and HZ condition: ($) P < 0.05, ($$) P < 0.01, ($$$) P < 0.001.
A correlation test was performed to assess whether hippocampal dysfunction was directly associated with cognitive impairment in 5xFAD transgenic mice (Table 2). Accumulation of Aβ (Aβ40, Aβ42, and total Aβ) was positively correlated to SachPT, time spent in open arms in EPM, and time spent to reach the platform in the phases of visual, acquisition and reversal spatial learning in MWM (P < 0.05). In contrast, SP, OPF and YMT were negatively related to Aβ accumulation (P < 0.05). As for neuroinflammatory markers, GFAP and Iba1 were positively related to SachPT and EPM, and negatively related to SPT (P < 0.05). We found correlations between pGSK3β and pmTOR expression and SachPT, SPT, and EPM (P < 0.05), which were significantly elevated in 10-12-month-old HZ mice. No correlations were found between MWM and neuroinflammatory markers and the PI3K-Akt signalling pathway, although only PTEN was negatively associated with acquisition and reversal spatial learning (P < 0.05), which was significantly elevated in 10-12-month-old HTZ and HZ mice.
3.7 Dysregulation of neuropeptide networks modulating insulin pathways in HTZ and HZ 5xFAD mice
Next, we evaluated insulin-linked metabolic pathways by monitoring plasma levels of peptides involved in modulating insulin release and signalling. Disruption of these neuropeptide networks could eventually contribute to the cognitive and behavioural disorders associated with AD (Cunnane et al., 2020; Robbins et al., 2020). Figure 5 shows the premature dysfunction of plasma hormone levels in 5xFAD transgenic mice, which is most notable at 4-6 months of age.
Insulin regulates metabolism by increasing cell permeability to monosaccharides, amino acids, and fatty acids. Two-way ANOVA revealed significant effects of genotype (F(2,75)= 7.136, P < 0.01) and age (F(1,75)= 4.946, P < 0.05). The 4–6-month-old HZ mice had low circulating plasma insulin levels compared to non-tg (Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 5A). These results were related to a reduction in the plasma level of GIP, which is a potent stimulator of insulin secretion. Statistical analysis showed that both HTZ and HZ mice showed low basal plasma levels of GIP compared to non-tg at 4-6 months of age (two-way ANOVA: age effect: F(1,75)= 18.42, P < 0.0001, Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 5B). Another peptide involved in the insulin-linked metabolic pathways is GLP-1, which has potent stimulatory effects on glucose-dependent insulin release. Two-way ANOVA revealed significant effects of genotype (F(2,75)= 3.777, P < 0.05) and age (F(1,75)= 10.37, P < 0.01). Post hoc comparison tests showed a significant decrease in basal plasma levels of GLP-1 in HZ mice, which was more marked at older age (Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 5C).
Leptin and ghrelin are two hormones implicated in the regulation of food intake and energy balance. Because the leptin system seems to be dependent on sexual dimorphism-dependent regulation (Quevedo et al., 1998; Roca et al., 1999), we analysed results by sex. We did not observe a dysregulation of plasma leptin levels between 5xFAD transgenic and no-transgenic mice (two-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, Figure 5D), whereas for plasma ghrelin levels there was a significant effect of age (two-way ANOVA: F(1,75)= 68.89, P < 0.0001). HZ mice showed lower circulating levels than HTZ mice at 4-6 months of age (Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 5E). Regarding circulating plasma glucagon levels, no significant differences were found between genotypes at any age (two-way ANOVA: P > 0.05, Figure 5F). Resistin is a peptide hormone produced by mature rodent adipocytes that regulates insulin sensitivity in both skeletal muscle and hepatic tissue (Steppan et al., 2001; Way et al., 2001) and appears to be associated with sexual dimorphism-dependent regulation (Gui et al., 2004). A ‘genotype x age x sex’ interaction was observed (two-way ANOVA: F(6,69)= 3.076, P < 0.01) in circulating plasma resistin levels. The post hoc analysis demonstrated a lower level of resistin in male and female HTZ mice than in non-tg and HZ mice at 4-6 months of age (Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 5G). PAI-1 is a serine protease inhibitor that regulates fibrinolysis, hence decreasing blood clot clearance. Two-way ANOVA demonstrated a significant effect of genotype (F(2,75)= 4.676, P < 0.05). Post hoc comparison analysis showed an increased PAI-1 levels in the HTZ group compared to non-tg mice (Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 5H).
Plasma levels of (A) insulin, (B) gastric inhibitory polypeptide (GIP), (C) glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), (D) leptin, (E) ghrelin, (F) glucagon, (G) resistin and (F) plasminogen activator inhibitor-1 (PAI-1). All data represent the mean ± SEM. Tukey’s Multiple Comparisons Test: difference between transgenic vs non-transgenic mice: (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01, (***) and P < 0.001; difference between HTZ and HZ condition: ($) P < 0.05.
To determine a relationship between cognitive impairment and the main changes found in plasma insulin-linked metabolic pathways, we performed a correlation test (Table 3). No correlations were found between insulin and GIP, and behaviour (P > 0.05). GLP-1 was positively related to spontaneous alternation behaviour in YMT, indicating low GLP-1 levels were effectively lower with decreased SAB. Whereas plasma levels of PAI-1 were negatively related to SAB in Y-maze test. Ghrelin was positively related to the visual learning phase in MWM (P < 0.05). Plasma levels of resistin were positively related to SPT and the reversal spatial learning phase in MWM, whereas they were negatively associated with SachPT (P < 0.05).
3.8 Decreased Firmicutes/Bacteroidetes ratio in 10–12-month-old HZ mice
The relative abundance of two main phyla of the gut microbiota composition and the Firmicutes/Bacteroidetes ratio were analysed. The relative abundance of Firmicutes phylum tended to decrease with age in the three genotypes (two-way ANOVA: age effect: F(1,62)= 98.67, P < 0.0001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 6A), while the abundance of Bacteroidetes phylum increased, being more prominent in middle-aged non-tg and HZ mice than in mature adult mice (two-way ANOVA: age effect: F(1,62)= 9.591, P < 0.001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 6B). Therefore, as a result, the Firmicutes/Bacteroidetes ratio was significantly reduced reaching the lowest values in HZ and non-tg groups at 10-12 months of age (two-way ANOVA: age effect: F(1,62)= 31.11, P < 0.0001, Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 6C) compared to those of 4-6 months of age. This trend was less pronounced in HTZ mice. Notably, no significant differences were found between the three genotypes in the two life stages analysed.
3.9 Principal Components Analysis revealed that factors other than Aβ accumulation increase the risk of developing AD
The relationship between cognitive impairment and pathophysiological alteration described above in the 5xFAD transgenic animal model at both 4-6 months and 10-12 months of age was subjected to PCA. The main results of the principal component analysis are given in Table 4. As a result, three components jointly explained 68.674% of the variance that allowed describing the different profiles of three genotypes (Figure 6B). The three dimensions and the Euclidean distances of each animal are shown in Figure 6C. The first component explained 29.975% of the total variance and was associated with Aβ accumulation since the results of the Aβ40, Aβ42 and total Aβ had high factor loading. Time spent in the open arms of EPM and neuroinflammation markers (GFAP and Iba1) loaded positively on this factor. The second component that explained 24.080% of the total variance was loaded by age and was therefore considered as the aging component. Insulin-linked metabolic pathways (resistin and ghrelin) and Firmicutes/Bacteroidetes ratio were negatively loaded, and the insulin-PI3K-Akt signalling pathway (pGSK3, pmTOR and PTEN) was positively loaded on this component. The third component, which explained 14.619%, represented memory impairment, as it was characterised by the phases of visual and acquisition learning in the MWM, the classic test for learning and memory. The insulin-PI3K-Akt signalling pathway (pIRS1) was also loaded positively on this component. Factor scores were then calculated and compared between groups. Two-way ANOVA revealed differences in the Aβ accumulation component (genotype effect: F(2,36)= 125.1, P < 0.0001) showing that this increase led to a changed in emotional-like behaviour and neuroinflammation in HTZ and HZ mice compared to non-tg mice (Tukey’s test: P < 0.05, Figure 6D). In addition, there was an age effect (two-way ANOVA: F(2,36)= 370.4, P < 0.0001) indicating that HZ mice performed worse on the aging component than HTZ and non-tg mice (Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 6E). For the memory impairment component, two-way ANOVA revealed a genotype effect (F(2,36)= 3.177, P < 0.05) showing differences between HTZ and HZ mice, which were more pronounced at 10-12 months of age (Tukey’s test: P ≤ 0.05, Figure 6F).
(A) Firmicutes and (B) Bacteroidetes phyla. (C) The Firmicutes/ Bacteroidetes ratio. (D) Exploratory principal component analysis (PCA). Three components (factors) together explained 68.674% of the variance associated with Aβ, aging, and memory impairment. (E) Diagram of the three components in rotated space. (F-H) PCA factorial scores. All data represent the mean ± SEM. Tukey’s Multiple Comparisons Test: difference between transgenic vs non-transgenic mice: (*) P < 0.05, (**) P < 0.01, (***) P < 0.001 and (****) P < 0.0001; difference between HTZ and HZ condition: ($) P < 0.05, ($$) P < 0.01.