In our study of U.S. of children from a general population sample of New Hampshire residents, we estimated associations between metals measured in infant toenails, a measure of perinatal metal exposure, and change in maladaptive behaviors from 3- to 5-years of age using repeated measures of children’s behavior. Overall, Cu was associated with improved behavior over time while Zn was associated with worse behavior over time, particularly externalizing behaviors. In secondary analyses both associations were more prominent among boys as compared to girls suggesting that boys may be more sensitive to metal-neurobehavior effects over time for this outcome and age range. Similarly, among boys, but not girls, Pb was associated with worse behavior from 3 to 5 years of age.
Problem behaviors assessed at 3 to 5 years of age are relatively stable in general populations as compared to earlier time points or outcomes (Bartels et al., 2004), consequently investigating this time frame may reduce variability from factors other than perinatal metal exposure. Characterizing maladaptive behavioral trajectories in early childhood (e.g., increasing internalizing or externalizing symptoms over time) could help to identify early indicators of risk for later psychopathology such as depression, anxiety or ADHD (Luby et al., 2019), potentially allowing for earlier intervention, mental health screening, or other preventative or ameliorative efforts (Wakschlag et al., 2010, 2019).
In our study infant toenails were used to estimate perinatal exposure to metals. Little research exists using infant toenails as biomarkers of metal exposures (Davis et al., 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2015). Toenails are composed of keratinous material rich in sulfhydrl groups, which bind metals, allowing toenails to be good potential biomatrices for metals exposure. They are easy to collect and store, and may reflect long term metals exposure due to their slow growth rate (Goullé et al., 2009; Gutiérrez-González et al., 2019; He, 2011). As for validity, evidence exists on the correlation between environmental/biological media and toenail concentrations of As, Mn, Se and Zn (Gutiérrez-González et al., 2019; Rodrigues et al., 2015; Signes-Pastor et al., 2021). Some evidence exists for toenail Pb to correlate with environmental dust in Pakistani urban and industrial areas (Mohmand et al., 2015) and in Zambian children living near mining industry (Ndilila et al., 2014). Toenail Pb has also been correlated with whole blood Pb in children exposed to higher Pb than the general country population in Ohio and Vietnam (Dantzer et al., 2020; Salcedo-Bellido et al., 2021; Sanders et al., 2014). However, toenail Pb was more weakly associated with cognitive outcomes as compared to blood Pb in Ohio children (Dantzer et al., 2020). Among the limited number of studies using toenail Cu as a biomarker of Cu exposure (Gutiérrez-González et al., 2019), toenail Cu has been correlated with indoor dust in populations living near to Cu-related industry (Berasaluce et al., 2019).
Comparisons from existing literature can be made to our study participant’s toenail metal concentrations. One study from Bangladesh with known water Mn and As contamination reported higher 1-month-old infant toenail metal concentrations than our U.S. rural general population cohort (median As: Rodrigues et al.: 0.5–0.7 µg/g vs. present study: 0.05 µg/g; Mn: Rodrigues et al.: 3.3–6.6 µg/g vs. present study: 1.0 µg/g). Another study of school-aged children (6–13 years) living in rural Canada with elevated Mn concentrations in their drinking water had twice the toenail Mn concentrations as our study (Ntihabose et al., 2018).
We observed a protective association of perinatal Cu with change in maladaptive behavioral symptoms from 3 to 5 years of age, which is consistent with the properties of Cu as an essential nutrient that supports brain development and possibly promotes neuroprotection (Bica et al., 2014; Hung et al., 2012; Kaler, 2011). However, findings remain mixed from previous studies in which Cu has been associated with enhanced (Jedynak et al. 2021; Liu et al. 2018), null (Polanska et al. 2017) or poor (Amorós et al. 2019a) neurobehavioral skills in children. Further, sparse research exists for estimating associations between metal mixtures, including Cu and neurodevelopmental trajectories. However, a recent Mexican study reported comparable findings to ours wherein higher second trimester maternal blood Cu was associated with improved cognition between ages 6 and 24 months, assessed with repeated measures of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, 3rd edition (BSID-III) (Liu et al., 2018). They observed an interaction between Cu and Pb, where the positive slope of the trajectory associated with Cu was lowest at high levels of Pb as compared to low levels of Pb. Our study did not observe interactions between metals. These differences may be due to use of different biomarkers of exposure (our study: infant toenails vs. second trimester blood (Liu et al.)) as well as assessment of different outcomes at different ages (maladaptive behavior from 3–5 years vs. cognition changes from 6–24 months). A prospective study of Spanish children found that first trimester serum Cu was associated with reduced cognition at 12 months and verbal ability at 5 years, although beta estimates were small in magnitude and cognitive assessments were different for the two time points (Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) vs. McCarthy Scales for Children) (Amorós et al., 2019). A Polish prospective birth cohort reported null associations between maternal plasma Cu measured at multiple perinatal time points and infant cognition assessed by the BSID at 12 or 24 months of age (Polanska et al., 2017). Collectively, the aforementioned research supports the need for more studies to investigate the protective and harmful effects of Cu over time and identify which Cu biomarkers may be most informative for children’s neurodevelopment.
We found that Zn was associated with an increase in maladaptive behaviors between 3 and 5 years, especially for externalizing behavior problems, although after removing influential points this association was weakened. While Zn is known for its beneficial effects on neurodevelopment (Adamo & Oteiza, 2010; Boscarino et al., 2021; Georgieff, 2007), recent prospective pediatric studies have observed harmful associations between increased Zn and children’s neurodevelopment (Horton et al., 2018; Polanska et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2013). A birth cohort study in Mexico City found that increased tooth Zn, reflecting exposures from birth to 11 months of age, was associated with more internalizing symptoms, but not with externalizing symptoms or BSI among 8-year-olds assessed with the BASC-2 (Horton et al., 2018). In contrast, our results were null for associations of perinatal metal exposures, including Zn, with internalizing symptoms. This may suggest that in the New Hampshire child population at 3–5 years, internalizing symptoms may not be sensitive to perinatal metals exposure. Or it may be outcome related, that metal associated changes in internalizing symptoms are more easily ascertained at older ages (Horton et al., participants’ mean age was 8 years). In terms of exposure, differences between our study and Horton et. al, include biomarkers and timing of exposure (infant 6-week toenails representing perinatal exposure in our study vs. deciduous teeth reflecting postnatal exposure for the first year of life in Horton et al., 2018). While these exposure time periods overlap around the neonatal period, they otherwise reflect different exposure time frames and potential routes of exposure; our perinatal metal toenail biomarker will additionally reflect maternal exposure, upregulation of nutrient absorption and placental transport of nutrient metals, while the tooth metal biomarker reflects postnatal exposures from birth to 11 months (Horton et al., 2018) and thereby incorporates postnatal infant hand-to-mouth behavior, introduction of solid foods and more infant formula use (Cohen Hubal et al., 2000; Ljung et al., 2011; Zota et al., 2016). Not only do metal exposures vary over the course of late pregnancy through the first of life, but so do neurodevelopmental processes. As such, susceptible time points of neurodevelopmental insult vary between these exposure time periods and could explain differences in our study findings and Horton et al., 2018 (Rice et al., 2000).
In exploratory sex-stratified analyses, associations of metals with longitudinal changes in behavior were typically more pronounced among boys as compared to girls. Among boys, perinatal Cu was associated with reduced problem behaviors over time, while Zn and Pb were associated with increased problem behaviors over time. It has been hypothesized that sex differences in metal-neurodevelopmental associations may be consequent to sex-specific anatomical and functional differences in brain development (Llop et al., 2013). For example, brain regions during development are sexually dimorphic and estrogens, which play a role in neuroprotection, are denser in some areas of developing female brains than in males (Rao & Kölsch, 2003; Scallet & Meredith, 2002; Vahter et al., 2002). In addition, male and female brains may rely on different neural pathways and information processing strategies to complete the same task (Cahill, 2006; Gillies et al., 2014). The few studies investigating sex as an effect modifier in the association between Cu and neurodevelopment reported more pronounced harmful associations between Cu and neurobehavior among boys(Amorós et al. 2019a; Kicinski et al. 2015; Polanska et al. 2017; Zhou et al. 2015) but there is one report of stronger Cu-associated decrements in visuospatial abilities in girls compared to boys (Rechtman et al. 2020). These findings generally support our observation that behavioral changes in boys were more sensitive to Cu than in girls (albeit a protective rather than harmful effect). A prospective study in Spain assessed prenatal Cu at multiple time points of cognitive development and reported an increase of 10 µg/L of 1st trimester maternal serum Cu was associated with poorer cognition on the BSID mental developmental index at 1 year among boys compared to improved performance in girls. Likewise at 5 years, Cu-associated decrements in cognition (using the verbal scale of the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities) were displayed in boys as compared to improved performance in girls, however sex differences were not statistically significant (Amorós et al. 2019a). Mixed findings have also been reported in multiple studies for sex differences in the association between Pb and children’s neurobehavior (Bauer et al. 2020b). For example, one study using three time points (12, 24 and 36 month) of the BSID reported stronger inverse associations between cord blood Pb and 36-month mental development index (MDI) among boys while null results were found in girls (Jedrychowski et al. 2009).
There are limitations for this study. The sample size was relatively small and therefore had limited statistical power to detect associations in sex-stratified analyses. For this reason, we presented these analyses as exploratory. We used one exposure metric for metals collected at one time point. However, unmeasured exposures at other critical developmental time periods may be more or less potent for child maladaptive behavior (Bauer et al. 2020b). This may be important especially when considering sex-specific associations. For example, a prospective Korean study reported sex- and exposure time-specific associations between blood Pb and total, internalizing and externalizing problems in 2 to 5-year-old children. Boys were more susceptible to prenatal Pb exposure, while girls were more susceptible to postnatal exposure (Joo et al. 2018). Using a repeat outcomes design allowed for exploration of changes in behavior over time, an outcome with potentially important functional implications but for which prior research is limited. While estimating metal-behavioral associations in early childhood is important in terms of understanding later mental health etiology, it is also possible that environmental metals are differently associated with behavioral symptoms at later time points of development (Luby et al., 2019; Wakschlag et al., 2010, 2019). For example, a US study of 6–8-year-old children also reported negative associations between Pb (measured in maternal erythrocytes) and behavioral difficulties, however Pb was more strongly associated with behavioral difficulties among girls as compared to boys (Fruh et al. 2019). Having access to only two time points for the outcome also hindered employment of new trajectory research methods (Liu et al. 2018). Other research studies examining associations of environmental metals on an outcome with more than two time points have used linear mixed models (Claus Henn et al., 2012; Coscia et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2018) or generalized estimating equations (Jedrychowski et al., 2009; Wasserman et al., 2000) to increase statistical power and allow for an overall estimate of the exposure effect across time and estimates at individual time points. Given that our data were only from two time points, using the change in BASC scores allowed us to investigate this trajectory question using both linear models as well as to initially explore metal mixture associations using BKMR with change in score as the outcome.
By using more than one time point for the outcome, our study contributes to understanding the potential role of metal exposures on neurodevelopmental trajectories, an area of sparse research (Braun et al. 2017). Neurodevelopmental trajectories reflect the progressive advancement of cognitive and behavioral skills that occurs over time as a child matures. Exposures during critical windows of development may change a trajectory’s course toward a more maladaptive or adaptive phenotype (Bellinger et al., 2016). Alterations in developmental trajectories, in turn, can be manifested as persistent deficits, developmental delays, or transient deficits (Rice & Barone Jr., 2000). Our findings support the potential for changes in behavior over time in young children to be informative for chemical exposure effects. In this context, we were able to identify metals that may improve, and others that may impair, behavioral progression in young children. If replicable, such findings could help guide interventions. In addition, given the potential for maladaptive behavioral trajectories in early childhood (e.g., our study’s observation of metal-associated increasing externalizing symptoms over time) to be an early indicator of risk for later psychopathology such as depression, anxiety or ADHD (Luby et al., 2019), assessment of behavioral changes over time would allow for earlier intervention, mental health screening, or other preventative or ameliorative efforts (Wakschlag et al., 2010, 2019).