Temporal changes in psychobehavioural responses during the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic in Malaysia

DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-24316/v1

Abstract

Monitoring public psychological and behavioural responses during the early phase of the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) outbreak is important for the management of infection prevention and control. This study aims to investigate the temporal trend in preventive measures against severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) infection, fear, socio-economic impact, perception of susceptibility/severity and anxiety levels during the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic. Between 25 January and 3 April 2020 a total of 962 survey responses were received. The majority of respondents had good levels of avoidance and protective behaviour. A high perception of severity (76.5%; 95%CI = 73.7–79.2) of SARS-CoV-2 infection was reported but perception of susceptibility was relatively low (30.6%; 95%CI = 27.7–33.6). A sizeable proportion of study participants were greatly impacted by the COVID-19 outbreak, with a total impact score of 9.1 (standard deviation 4.7) from a possible total score of 15, and 72.1% (95%CI = 69.2–75.0) reported moderate to severe anxiety as measured by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Overall, the psychological and behavioural responses were found to increase with progression of the outbreak, however a slightly higher increase was observed for the socio-economic impact. These temporal trends during the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic provide valuable insights for mitigation strategies to contain the current rapidly escalating outbreak.


Introduction

An outbreak of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), caused by severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2), was initially detected in Wuhan, China in December 2019. In less than one month the outbreak has since resulted in an epidemic throughout China (Hui et al., 2020; Lu et al., 2020). The new coronavirus rapidly spread across Asian countries and subsequently all around the world within a month of its onset. Malaysia announced the first three cases of SARS-CoV-2 infection on 25 January 2020. At a later date, with increasing cases, various measures were undertaken by the Malaysian government to contain the spread of COVID-19, including extending travel bans for nationals from Iran, Italy and South Korea. On 17 March 2020 the number of confirmed cases in Malaysia had reached 673; this is also the date when the first death was reported and the country implemented a nationwide movement control order (MCO) to curb the outbreak.

Lessons from previous outbreaks, such as SARS, swine flu (H1N1) and Ebola, have gained international recognition for the importance of understanding and monitoring the lay public’s psychobehavioural responses during infectious disease outbreaks. Monitoring of  psychobehavioural response, particularly during the early phase of an outbreak, plays an important role in identifying behavioural gaps and provides important insights for immediate intervention, helping to bring the outbreak under control (Opare et al., 2000; Leung et al., 2005; Leppin and Aro, 2009). Empirical studies have provided some conceptual-theoretical frameworks regarding psychobehavioural responses during infectious disease outbreaks. For instance, during the SARS outbreaks, a positive association between anxiety level and personal protective behaviour was found (Seto et al., 2003; Leung et al., 2005). During infectious disease outbreaks, tension may surge in the entire community and result in a significant disruption of general well-being, health and economy (Wishnick, 2010; Qiu et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2019). It is important to note that the public often judges personal risk based on their impressions of disease prevalence and death rates, therefore assessment of risk perception is important and changes in risk perception throughout the epidemic warrant appropriate understanding (Herrera and Meyers, 2019). Most recently, with the current COVID-19 outbreak, the psychological impact of the outbreak intensified when the community was placed under controlled movement or quarantine (Brooks et al., 2020).

Notwithstanding the prominent psychobehavioural consequences during a disease outbreak, to what extent epidemiologists and social scientists have understood the psychobehavioural aspect of the current COVID-19 outbreak in Malaysia is unknown. In the previous 2009 H1N1 pandemic, epidemic-related psychobehavioural responses of the Malaysian public were reported (Wong and Sam, 2010) and it is timely that a similar study should be carried out during the current COVID-19 outbreak. This study therefore aims to investigate psychobehavioural issues related to the COVID-19 outbreak, namely preventive measures against SARS-CoV-2 infection, fear, impact, perception of susceptibility/severity and anxiety levels. Likewise, this study also examines the temporal changes in psychobehavioural effects by following the psychobehavioural responses from the day of the first reported COVID-19 case until initiation of the MCO by the Malaysian government. These findings will provide insights for the formulation of mitigation measures to manage the negative impact in a timely manner.

Method

An anonymous Internet-based, cross-sectional survey was commenced on 25 January 2020, the day the first case of SARS-CoV-2 was reported in Malaysia. The survey was carried out over a period of 10 weeks up until 3 April 2020 and the responses were divided into three time periods of around 3 weeks: 25 January–21 February, 22 February–17 March and 18 March–3 April (the period of the MCO). The pyschobehavioural responses were compared among the respondents from these three groups. Inclusion criteria were that the respondents were from the general public of Malaysia and aged 18–70 years. The researchers used social network platforms (Facebook, Instragram and Whatsapp) to disseminate and advertise the survey link. Those respondents who completed the survey received a note to encourage them to disseminate the survey link to all their contacts. All respondents were informed that their participation was voluntary and consent was implied through their completion of the questionnaire.

The questionnaire was developed in English and then translated into Bahasa Malaysia, the national language of Malaysia. Questions were presented in both English and Bahasa Malaysia. Local experts validated the content of the questionnaire and then it was pilot tested. The survey consisted of questions that assessed: demographic background; preventive measures against SARS-CoV-2 infection; perceived susceptibility and severity of becoming infected with SARS-CoV-2; 4) fear associated with SARS-CoV-2 infection; socio-economic impact; and anxiety level associated with the COVID-19 outbreak.

Questions on demographic characteristics assessed the participants’ age, gender, ethnicity, highest educational level, average household income and locality. Questions on prevention practices were divided into two sections (11 items): avoidance behaviours (5 items) and protective behaviours (6 items). Questions on perceived susceptibility (1 item) and severity (1 item) asked participants to rate their level of likelihood of becoming infected with SARS-CoV-2 and their level of worry over SARS-CoV-2 infection, respectively. Both of these questions were based on constructs from the Health Belief Model (Becker, 1974; Rosenstock, 1974; Champion and Skinner, 2008). Perceived fear and socio-economic impact consisted of 4 items and 5 items, respectively. The response options for prevention practices, fear and impact were scored on a four-point Likert scale: 1, strongly disagree; 2, disagree; 3, agree; 4, strongly agree. The response options for perceived susceptibility and severity were also scored on a four-point Likert scale: 1, not at all; 2, somewhat; 3, very; 4, extremely. The scores were summed, with higher scores representing higher prevention measures, perceived severity/susceptibility, fear and impact. Apart from the questions on perception of susceptibility/severity, all the questions were adopted from a previous study (Wong and Sam, 2010).

Anxiety was measured using the 6-item state version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-6: Marteau and Bekker, 1992; Hou et al., 2015). Respondents rated the frequency of experiencing six emotional states, namely being calm, tense, upset, relaxed, content and worried, connected with the current COVID-19 outbreak. A four-point scale was used (1, not at all; 2, somewhat; 3, moderately; 4, very much). The scores on the three positively worded items were reverse coded. The total summed scores were pro-rated (multiplied by 20/6) to obtain scores that were comparable with those from the full 20-item STAI (giving a range of 20–80) (Marteau and Bekker, 1992). A cut-off score of 44 was used to indicate moderate to severe symptoms (Knight et al., 1983; Leung et al., 2005).

Statistical analysis

The reliability of the scales used was evaluated by assessing the internal consistency of the items representing the scores. The avoidance and protective behaviour items had a reliability (Cronbach’s α) of 0.921 and 0.707, respectively. The reliability of the fear and impact items was 0.779 and 0.906, respectively. The reliability computed for the STAI-6 items in the assessment of anxiety was 0.781. Multivariable logistic regression analysis, using a simultaneous forced-entry method, was used to determine the factors influencing moderate to severe anxiety. Odds ratios (OR), 95% confidence intervals (95%CI) and p values were calculated for each independent variable.

All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, version 20.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). The level of significance was set at p < 0.05.

Ethical considerations

This research was approved by the University of Malaya Research Ethics Committee (UM.TNC2/UMREC – 847).

Results

A total of 962 completed responses were received. As shown in the first and second columns of Table 1, the mean age ± standard deviation (SD) of the study participants is 35.5 ± 11.2 years. The study has a slightly higher representation of females (68.6%), participants with tertiary education level (91.3%) and from the Central region (67.7%). With regard to ethnicity, the majority were Malay (58.7%), followed by Chinese (29.3%). Figure 1 illustrates the proportions of each pyschobehavioural response and Figure 2 displays the trend of these responses according to the three time periods.                         

 

Table 1 Socio demography, fear, impact, prevention behavior and anxiety level of COVID-19 (N=962)

 

Overall

n(%)

25th Jan – 21st Feb

(n=581)

22nd Feb-

17th March

(n=71)

18th March –

3rd April

(n=310)

p-value

Socio demography

 

 

 

 

 

Age group (mean±SD)

35.5±11.2

34.7±10.6

33.1±14.8

37.5±11.1

 

Gender

 

 

 

 

 

  Male

302 (31.4)

156 (26.9)

21 (29.6)

125 (40.3)

 

  Female

660 (68.6)

425 (73.1)

50 (70.4)

185 (59.7)

 

Ethnicity

 

 

 

 

 

  Malay

565 (58.7)

280 (48.2)

30 (42.3)

255 (82.3)

 

  Chinese

282 (29.3)

216 (37.2)

40 (56.3)

26 (8.4)

 

  Indian

53 (5.5)

38 (6.5)

1 (1.4)

14 (4.5)

 

  Bumiputera Sabah/Sarawak/Others

62 (6.4)

47 (8.1)

0 (0.0)

15 (4.8)

 

Level of education

 

 

 

 

 

  Secondary school

84 (8.7)

28 (4.8)

5 (7.0)

51 (16.5)

 

  Tertiary

878 (91.3)

553 (95.2)

66 (93.0)

259 (83.5)

 

Occupation

 

 

 

 

 

  Professional and managerial

551 (57.3)

368 (63.3)

21 (29.6)

162 (52.3)

 

  General worker

131 (13.6)

52 (9.0)

6 (8.5)

73 (23.5)

 

  Student

173 (18.0)

109 (18.8)

37 (52.1)

27 (8.7)

 

  Housewife/Retiree/ Unemployed/Others

107 (11.1)

52 (9.0)

7 (9.9)

48 (15.5)

 

Average monthly household income (MYR)

 

 

 

 

 

<2000

97 (10.1)

48 (8.3)

19 (26.8)

30 (9.7)

 

2001-4000

308 (32.0)

172 (29.6)

15 (21.1)

121 (39.0)

 

4001-8000

311 (32.3)

193 (33.2)

15 (21.1)

103 (33.2)

 

>8000

246 (25.6)

168 (28.9)

22 (31.0)

56 (18.1)

 

Location

 

 

 

 

 

Urban

657 (68.3)

436 (75.0)

47 (66.2)

174 (56.1)

 

Sub urban/Rural

305 (31.7)

145 (25.0)

24 (33.8)

136 (43.9)

 

Region

 

 

 

 

 

Central

651 (67.7)

401 (69.0)

42 (59.2)

208 (67.1)

 

Southern

71 (7.4)

42 (7.2)

5 (7.0)

24 (7.7)

 

Northern

89 (9.3)

53 (9.1)

7 (9.9)

29 (9.4)

 

East Coast

74 (7.7)

22 (3.8)

15 (21.1)

37 (11.9)

 

Borneo Island

77 (8.0)

63 (10.8)

2 (2.8)

12 (3.9)

 

Prevention behaviour

 

 

 

 

 

Mean total avoidance behavior score (mean±SD)

12.1±3.5

11.2±3.7

11.8±3.2

13.9±2.2

p<0.001

Mean total protective behavior score (mean±SD)

15.0±2.8

14.6±3.0

14.5±2.7

16.0±2.1

p<0.001

Fear of COVID-19

 

 

 

 

 

Mean total fear score (mean±SD)

10.0±2.3

9.6±2.4

9.3±2.5

10.9±1.7

p<0.001

Impact from COVID-19

 

 

 

 

 

Mean total impact score (mean±SD)

9.1±4.7

7.6±4.7

9.2±4.8

11.9±2.9

p<0.001

Perceived susceptibility

 

 

 

 

 

   Very/Extremely

294 (30.6)

26.3

(95%CI 22.8-30.1)

18.3

(95%CI 10.1-29.3)

58.7

(95%CI 53.0-64.2)

p<0.001

Perceived severity

 

 

 

 

 

   Very/Extremely

736 (76.5)

72.6

(95%CI 68.8-76.2)

70.4

(95%CI 58.4-80.7)

85.2%

(95%CI 80.7-88.8)

p<0.001

Anxiety

 

 

 

 

 

Total anxiety score

52.0±13.0

50.9±13.2

52.1±12.7

54.0±12.5

0.003

Moderate/Severe (score 44-80)

694 (72.1)

69.0

(95%CI 65.1-72.8)

74.6

(95%CI 62.9-84.2)

77.4

(95%CI 72.4-82.0)

0.026

 Central: Selangor, Kuala Lumpur, Putrajaya, Negeri Sembilan, Malacca; Southern: Johor; Northern: Perlis, Kedah, Pulau Pinang, Perak; East Coast: Terengganu, Kelantan, Pahang; Borneo Island: Sabah, Sarawak, Labuan

 

Avoidance behaviour

Figure 1.1 shows the proportion of strongly agree/agree responses for avoidance behaviour in the three successive time periods. All the avoidance behaviour items showed an increase in the proportion of strongly agree/agree across the three time periods. The highest avoidance behaviour was in relation to ‘avoid going abroad’. The overall total avoidance behaviour score was 12.1 ± 3.5 out of a possible range of 0–15. The median was 13 (interquartile range IQR = 10–15). The avoidance behaviour scores were categorized as a score of 13–15 or 0–12 based on the median split; thus, 558 (58.0%; 95%CI = 54.8–61.1) were categorized as having a score of 13–15 and 404 (42.0%; 95%CI = 38.8–45.2) with a score of 0–12. Statistically significant differences were observed in the increase in mean total avoidance behaviour score for respondents during 25 January–21 February (11.2 ± 3.7), 22 February–17 March (11.8 ± 3.2) and 18 March–3 April (13.9 ± 2.2).

 

Protective behaviour

Figure 1.2 shows the proportion of strongly agree/agree responses for the three successive time periods. All the avoidance behaviour items showed an increase in the proportion of strongly agree/agree across the three time periods. The highest increment was seen for wearing a mask out in public for 18 March–3 April. The overall total protective behaviour score was 15.0 ± 2.8 out of the possible range 0–18. The total protective behaviour score of the responses in this study ranged from 4 to 18. The median was 16 (IQR = 13–17). The protective behaviour scores were categorized as a score of 16–18 or 4–15 based on the median split: thus, 524 (54.5%; 95%CI = 51.3–57.7) were categorized as having a score of 16–18 and 438 (45.5%; 95%CI = 42.3–48.7) with a score of 4–15. There is a statistically significant difference in the increase in the mean total protective behaviour score for respondents during 25 January–21 February (14.6 ± 3.0), 22 February–17 March (14.5 ± 2.7), and 18 March–3 April (16.0 ± 2.1).

Fear

Figure 1.3 shows that across the three time periods the highest level of fear was for eating wildlife animals whereas the lowest level was for eating out. The overall total fear score was 10.0 ± 2.3 out of a possible range of 0–12. The median was 11 (IQR = 9–12). The fear scores were categorized as a score of 11–18 or 0–10 based on the median split: thus, 511 (53.1%; 95%CI = 49.9–56.3) were categorized as having a score of 11–12 and 451 (46.9%; 95%CI = 43.7–50.1) with a score of 0–10. The mean total score for respondents during 18 March–3 April was significantly higher than those of the earlier time periods.

 

Impact

Figure 1.4 shows that the highest impact was on the family’s daily routine. All the impact items showed an increase in proportion of strongly agree/agree across the three time periods. The overall total impact score was 9.1 ± 4.7 out of the possible range of 0–15. The median was 10 (IQR = 6–13). The impact scores were categorized as a score of 10–15 or 0–9 based on the median split: thus, 509 (52.9%; 95%CI = 49.7–56.1) were categorized as having a score of 10–15 and 453 (47.1%; 95%CI = 43.9–50.3) with a score of 0–12. There is a statistically significant increase in the mean total impact score for respondents during 25 January–21 February 21 (7.6 ± 4.7), 22 February–17 March (9.2 ± 4.8), and 18 March–3 April (11.9 ± 2.9.) Of note, Figure 2 shows that among all the pyschobehavioural variables the trend line of the total impact scores showed a slightly greater upward slope over time.

 

Perception of susceptibility and severity

Respondents during 18 March–3 April reported a higher proportion of extremely/very responses for perception of susceptibility (41.3%) to becoming infected with SARS-CoV-2 compared to those of 25 January–21 February (26.3%) and 22 February–17 March  (18.3%). Likewise, respondents during 18 March–3 April reported a higher proportion of extremely/very responses for perception of severity (83.2%) of SARS-CoV-2 infection compared to those of 25 January–21 February (70.4%) and 22 February–17 March (72.6%). On the whole, a higher proportion of participants expressed higher susceptibility than severity of SARS-CoV-2 infection across the three time periods.

 

Anxiety levels

Using a cut-off score of 44 for the STAI score, a total of 72.1% (n = 694, 95%CI = 69.2–75.0) of the overall respondents reported moderate to severe anxiety. The proportion of respondents with moderate to severe anxiety during 25 January–21 February, 22 February–17 March and 18 March–3 April was 69.0% (95%CI = 65.1–72.8), 74.6% (95%CI = 62.9–84.2) and 77.4% (95%CI = 72.4–82.0), respectively. All the psychobehavioural variables were found to be significantly associated with anxiety levels in the univariate analyses. However, none of the demographic characteristics were significantly associated with anxiety levels. There was an inverse association between income and level of anxiety, although the association is not significant. Respondents with income below MYR2000 reported the highest proportion having moderate and severe anxiety (75.3%), followed by MYR2001–4000 (73.4%), MYR4001–8000 (71.1%) and above MYR8000 (70.7%; p = 0.771). A total of 73.9% females reported having moderate to severe anxiety compared to only 68.2% in males (p = 0.075). For ethnicity, a higher proportion of Indians reported moderate to severe anxiety (77.4%) compared to only 74.2% among the Malays and 69.1% among the Chinese (p = 0.124).

Table 2 shows the multivariable logistic regression analysis of factors associated with moderate to severe anxiety. In the multivariate model, the most important factor influencing moderate to severe anxiety is a high perception of severity (OR = 2.09; 95%CI = 1.48–2.94), followed by high perceived susceptibility (OR = 1.71; 95%CI = 1.17–2.50), high impact score (OR = 1.63; 95%CI = 1.17–2.26) and high fear score (OR = 1.47; 95%CI = 1.01–2.14).

 

 

 

Table 2 Multivariable logistic regression analysis of factors associated with moderate to severe anxiety (N=962)

 

 

Univariate

Multivariable logistic regression

 

Frequency (%)

Moderate/Severe anxiety

(Score 44-80)

Score 44-80 vs 20-43

OR (95CI%)

Perceived susceptibility

 

 

 

Likelihood to be infected by COVID-19

 

 

 

  Not at all/Somewhat

668 (69.4)

446 (66.8)

Ref

  Very/Extremely

294 (30.6)

248 (84.4)

1.71 (1.17-2.50)**

Perceived severity

 

 

 

Worry about consequences from COVID-19

 

 

 

  Not at all/Somewhat

226 (23.5)

119 (52.7)

Ref

  Very/Extremely

736 (76.5)

575 (78.1)

2.09 (1.48-2.94)***

Fear

 

 

 

  Score 0-10

451 (46.9)

283 (62.7)

Ref

  Score 11-12

511 (53.1)

411 (80.4)

1.47 (1.01-2.14)*

Impact

 

 

 

  Score 0-9

453 (47.1)

283 (62.5)

Ref

  Score 10-15

509 (52.9)

411 (80.7)

1.63 (1.17-2.26)**

Avoidance behavior 

 

 

 

Score 0-12

404 (42.0)

257 (63.6)

Ref

  Score 13-15

558 (58.0)

437 (78.3)

1.06 (0.73-1.53)

Protective behaviour

 

 

 

Score 4-15

438 (45.5)

285 (65.1)

Ref

  Score 16-18

524 (54.5)

409 (78.1)

1.18 (0.85-1.65)

*p<0.05**p<0.01, ***p<0.001

Hosmer–Lemeshow test, chi-square: 3.985, p-value: 0.858; Nagelkerke R2 : 0.134

Discussion

In view of the urgency and importance of identifying current psychobehavioural issues, as well as providing information for those segments of the population that most need psychobehavioural interventions, our findings are summarized over a period of 10 weeks from the start of data collection and include 2 weeks when the country was under an MCO. We hope that the results from this study will provide insights for the health authorities to use when developing risk communication messages during the current escalating COVID-19 epidemic in Malaysia, so that appropriate intervention can be carried out to help curb the current escalating outbreak and also ensure the well-being of the public while the outbreak is ongoing.

In general, the study respondents during 18 March–3 April, which covers 2 weeks of the MCO, have a higher level of avoidance and protective behaviour compared to respondents in the two earlier periods of the study. The study identified some gaps in preventive measures, the most important being the wearing of masks. Less than one-third of participants before the MCO period were found to wear masks when they were out in public. Nearly 27% reported not wearing masks when out in public during the MCO period, which deserves serious attention. In view of the SARS-CoV-2 infection being extremely contagious, our findings underscore the importance of public health intervention to reach individuals with poor adherence to preventive measures, especially the wearing of masks. Earlier, the World Health Organization (WHO) recommended against wearing masks in community settings as no evidence is available for its usefulness (WHO, 2020a). However, its latest recommendation advises wearing face masks in public areas where social distancing can be difficult (WHO, 2020b), and this should be made known to the public. It is widely known that the shortage of face masks could be the reason why people are not using them, therefore the public should be advised to use another form of facial protection, such as cloth masks, when a face mask is not available. Although they are not as effective as surgical masks, research suggests that they can limit droplet transmission from infected individuals (Davies et al., 2013).

It is important for the public to have a certain level of fear towards the COVID-19 outbreak in order to enhance protective behaviour. Previous SARS epidemics and the 2015 Middle East Respiratory Syndrome (MERS) outbreak revealed that people’s fear, anxiety and infection sensitivity appear to have a positive impact by trigger care-seeking and personal health protection (Leung et al., 2003; Chang et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2016). Importantly, this study has found that the feeling of fear was relatively low among the study respondents, particularly the fear of going to public eateries from the early phase of the COVID-19 outbreak up to and during the MCO period. Community messages encouraging people to stay at home, avoid visiting public places, practise social distancing and pack their food instead of eating outside have been widely circulated since the beginning of the MCO period. The public therefore should be enlightened to avoid public places, as it has been widely recommended that sustainable social distancing and rigorous implementation of social distancing are needed to contain the COVID-19 outbreak (Wilder-Smith et al., 2020).

Results from this study also indicated that a sizeable proportion of study participants were greatly impacted by the COVID-19 outbreak, particularly in their family’s daily routine and both non-work and work-related travel. Although official data on the economic impact of COVID-19 in Malaysia are not yet available, nationwide movement control has had a big impact on work-related travel and jobs, as found in this study. At the moment, with slightly over 2 weeks of the MCO, the financial impact is approaching 50%, indicating the potential catastrophic impact of COVID-19 to the economics of Malaysia in the coming future. There is thus an urgent need to mitigate any potential economic fallout of the coronavirus economic crisis. Furthermore, the trend line of the total impact scores showed a slightly greater upward slope over time compared to other psychobehavioural variables, implying increasing social and economic impact as the outbreak progresses. These findings warrant immediate mitigations to strengthen community resilience in order to endure these impacts during the COVID-19 outbreak.

Risk perception, or an individual’s perceived susceptibility to a threat, is an important determinant of protective behaviour. Responses to the H1N1 influenza outbreak in 2009 showed that the success of public health intervention programmes is dependent on individual risk perception (Ibuka et al., 2010; Carlsen and Glenton, 2016). Importantly, the perceived severity of SARS-CoV-2 infection is higher than the perception of susceptibility. Since the beginning of the COVID-19 epidemic in the epicentre, Wuhan, the escalating confirmed cases and deaths from coronavirus in China have received extensive coverage in Malaysia. Of note, this survey commences on the day that the first case of SARS-CoV-2 infection was reported. Despite extensive media coverage and even after the first confirmed case in Malaysia, the perception of susceptibility remains low among participants.

The findings of over two-thirds of the study respondents suffering moderate to severe anxiety indicated that COVID-19 has a strong influence on the mental health of the people of Malaysia. A recent study conducted during the COVID-19 outbreak in China reported that females experience higher psychological impact than males (Wang et al., 2020) and a higher proportion of females reported moderate to severe anxiety than males; nevertheless, these differences were not significant. The current study is unable to provide recommendations on high-risk demographic groups for psychological intervention as there was no significant association between anxiety level and respondents’ demographic characteristics. Future research involving a larger sample size is needed to further explore the potential socio-demographic differences in psychological impact associated with COVID-19.

The finding of an increased proportion of respondents with a moderate to severe perception of severity over time is worrisome. Currently, the number of COVID-19 cases is still increasing in Malaysia and the pandemic has yet to reach its peak. This may indicate that a higher proportion of the public may experience anxiety disorders as the epidemic progresses. Multivariable analysis showed that having a high perception of severity and susceptibility is an important determinant of a high anxiety level. The public is more likely to express higher anxiety levels if they perceive they are at risk of becoming infected with SARS-CoV-2: for instance, if someone in their community is infected. This finding supports the hypothesis that risk perception is higher when a health threat is high, uncontrollable or dreaded (Ferrer and Klein, 2015). Previous studies have reported the benefit of a higher perception of susceptibility and severity in triggering a higher practice of recommended prevention measures during disease outbreak (Lau et al, 2005; Leung et al., 2005; Rubin et al., 2009). There is therefore a need to develop appropriate public educational messages that impart accurate information about the new COVID-19 outbreak yet at the same time do not overestimate the likelihood of infection or underestimate the risk of infection.

Furthermore, in this study the increase in anxiety among those with high socio-economic impact scores is anticipated. The COVID-19 outbreak has affected both the economic and social well-being of the public. On the whole, our findings support the crucial need for the development and implementation of mental health support, treatment and services for the public in Malaysia during the current COVID-19 outbreak (Xiang et al., 2020). It is particularly important for the public to maintain a moderate level of anxiety, as extreme anxiety may impair immune system functioning and increase the risk of infection (Marshall, 2011).

As with all studies, it is worth noting a few limitations of the present study, particularly concerning the study design and data collection method. Firstly, due to the cross-sectional methodology, the directionality of the association or the causal relationship, risk perception, fear, impact and anxiety levels could not be established; however, the findings provide a basis for acquiring and testing a causal hypothesis. Due to various resource limitations during the disease crisis and movement restriction in Malaysia, using an online web-based questionnaire via a social media platform may lead to selection bias, as reflected in the large sample of females and the majority being from the Central region. Despite the lack of representativeness, the current study provides useful first-hand information on public psychobehavioural responses during the new COVID-19 outbreak.

Conclusion

Overall, the public psychological and behavioural responses were found to increase with progression of the COVID-19 outbreak. The majority have a high level of preventive measures and perception of severity of SARS-CoV-2 infection but perception of susceptibility is relatively low. A sizeable proportion of study participants were greatly impacted by the COVID-19 outbreak in their family’s daily routine and both non-work and work-related travel. The larger increase in social and economic impact as the outbreak progresses warrants immediate mitigations to strengthen community resilience in order to endure these impacts during the COVID-19 outbreak. A high anxiety level was observed during all three time periods. The factor influencing higher anxiety was a high perception of severity and susceptibility, impact and fear. This timely psychobehavioural assessment during the early phase of the current COVID-19 epidemic among the Malaysian public is valuable for providing insights into strategies for health risk communication and mental health intervention as the COVID-19 pandemic progresses.

Declarations

Acknowledgements

We are grateful for the time that the participants gave to this study.

Funding

None

Authors contribution

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by Li Ping Wong and Haridah Alias. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Li Ping Wong and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee University of Malaya Research Ethics Committee (UM.TNC2/UMREC – 847).

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

References

Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and personal health behavior. Health education monographs2, 324-473.

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., & Rubin, G. J. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet, 395 (10227):912-920. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8.

Carlsen, B., & Glenton, C. (2016). The swine flu vaccine, public attitudes, and researcher interpretations: a systematic review of qualitative research. BMC health services research16(1), 203.  https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-016-1466-7.

Champion, V. L., & Skinner, C. S. (2008). The health belief model. Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice4, 45-65.

 Chang, H. J., Huang, N., Lee, C. H., Hsu, Y. J., Hsieh, C. J., & Chou, Y. J. (2004). The impact of the SARS epidemic on the utilization of medical services: SARS and the fear of SARS. American journal of public health94(4), 562-564.  doi: 10.2105/ajph.94.4.562.

Davies, A., Thompson, K. A., Giri, K., Kafatos, G., Walker, J., & Bennett, A. (2013). Testing the efficacy of homemade masks: would they protect in an influenza pandemic?. Disaster medicine and public health preparedness7(4), 413-418. DOI: 10.1017/dmp.2013.43

Ferrer, R. A., & Klein, W. M. (2015). Risk perceptions and health behavior. Current opinion in psychology5, 85-89. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.03.012.

Herrera, J. L., & Meyers, L. A. (2019). Local risk perception enhances epidemic control. Plos One, 14 (2): e0225576.

Hou, W. K., Hall, B. J., Canetti, D., Lau, K. M., Ng, S. M., & Hobfoll, S. E. (2015). Threat to democracy: Physical and mental health impact of democracy movement in Hong Kong. Journal of Affective Disorders186, 74-82. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.07.005

Hui, D. S., I Azhar, E., Madani, T. A., Ntoumi, F., Kock, R., Dar, O., ... & Zumla, A. (2020). The continuing 2019-nCoV epidemic threat of novel coronaviruses to global health—The latest 2019 novel coronavirus outbreak in Wuhan, China. International Journal of Infectious Diseases91, 264-266.

Ibuka, Y., Chapman, G. B., Meyers, L. A., Li, M., & Galvani, A. P. (2010). The dynamics of risk perceptions and precautionary behavior in response to 2009 (H1N1) pandemic influenza. BMC infectious diseases10(1), 296. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2334-10-296

Knight, R. G., Waal‐Manning, H. J., & Spears, G. F. (1983). Some norms and reliability data for the State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Zung Self‐Rating Depression scale. British Journal of Clinical Psychology22, 245-249. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1983.tb00610.x

Lau, J. T., Yang, X., Pang, E., Tsui, H. Y., Wong, E., & Wing, Y. K. (2005). SARS-related perceptions in Hong Kong. Emerging infectious diseases11(3), 417. doi: 10.3201/eid1103.040675

Lee, S. Y., Yang, H. J., Kim, G., Cheong, H. K., & Choi, B. Y. (2016). Preventive behaviors by the level of perceived infection sensitivity during the Korea outbreak of Middle East Respiratory Syndrome in 2015. Epidemiology and health38: e2016051. DOI: 10.4178/epih.e2016051. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28092927/]

Leppin, A., & Aro, A. R. (2009). Risk perceptions related to SARS and avian influenza: theoretical foundations of current empirical research. International journal of behavioral medicine16(1), 7-29. DOI: 10.1007/s12529-008-9002-8

Leung, G. M., Ho, L. M., Chan, S. K., Ho, S. Y., Bacon-Shone, J., Choy, R. Y., ... & Fielding, R. (2005). Longitudinal assessment of community psychobehavioral responses during and after the 2003 outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. Clinical Infectious Diseases40, 1713-1720. doi.org/10.1086/429923

Leung, G. M., Lam, T. H., Ho, L. M., Ho, S. Y., Chan, B. H. Y., Wong, I. O. L., & Hedley, A. J. (2003). The impact of community psychological responses on outbreak control for severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health57(11), 857-863. DOI: 10.1136/jech.57.11.857.

Lu, H., Stratton, C. W., & Tang, Y. W. (2020). Outbreak of Pneumonia of Unknown Etiology in Wuhan China: the Mystery and the Miracle. Journal of Medical Virology. 92. DOI:10.1002/jmv.25678.

Marshall, G. D. (2011). The adverse effects of psychological stress on immunoregulatory balance: applications to human inflammatory diseases. Immunology and Allergy Clinics31(1), 133-140. doi: 10.1016/j.iac.2010.09.013.

Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six‐item short‐form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British journal of clinical Psychology31(3), 301-306. doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00997.x

Opare, C., Nsiire, A., Awumbilla, B., & Akanmori, B. D. (2000). Human behavioural factors implicated in outbreaks of human anthrax in the Tamale municipality of northern Ghana. Acta tropica76(1), 49-52. DOI: 10.1016/S0001-706X(00)00089-9

Qiu, W., Chu, C., Mao, A., & Wu, J. (2018). The Impacts on Health, Society, and Economy of SARS and H7N9 Outbreaks in China: A Case Comparison Study. Journal of environmental and public health2018. doi: 10.1155/2018/2710185.

Rosenstock, I. M. (1974). Historical origins of the health belief model. Health education monographs2(4), 328-335. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019817400200403.

Rubin, G. J., Amlôt, R., Page, L., & Wessely, S. (2009). Public perceptions, anxiety, and behaviour change in relation to the swine flu outbreak: cross sectional telephone survey. Bmj339, b2651. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2651

Seto, W. H., Tsang, D., Yung, R. W. H., Ching, T. Y., Ng, T. K., Ho, M., ... & Advisors of Expert SARS group of Hospital Authority. (2003). Effectiveness of precautions against droplets and contact in prevention of nosocomial transmission of severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS). The Lancet361(9368), 1519-1520. DOI: 10.1016/s0140-6736(03)13168-6

Smith, K. M., Machalaba, C. C., Seifman, R., Feferholtz, Y., & Karesh, W. B. (2019). Infectious disease and economics: The case for considering multi-sectoral impacts. One Health7, 100080. doi: 10.1016/j.onehlt.2018.100080

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in china. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health17(5), 1729. DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17051729.

WHO. (2020a, January 29). Advice on the use of masks in the community, during home care and in health care settings in the context of the novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV) outbreak: interim guidance. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/330987

WHO.(2020b, April 6). Advice on the use of masks in the context of COVID-19. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/publications-detail/advice-on-the-use-of-masks-in-the-community-during-home-care-and-in-healthcare-settings-in-the-context-of-the-novel-coronavirus-(2019-ncov)-outbreak

Wilder-Smith, A., Chiew, C. J., & Lee, V. J. (2020). Can we contain the COVID-19 outbreak with the same measures as for SARS?. The Lancet Infectious Diseases. Published online 2020, March 5. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30129-8.
Wishnick E. (2010) Dilemmas of securitization and health risk management in the People’s Republic of China: The cases of SARS and avian influenza. Health Policy Plan, 25:454-66. doi:10.1093/heapol/czq065.

Wong, L. P., & Sam, I. C. (2010). Temporal changes in psychobehavioral responses during the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic. Preventive medicine51(1), 92-93. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.010.

Xiang, Y. T., Yang, Y., Li, W., Zhang, L., Zhang, Q., Cheung, T., & Ng, C. H. (2020). Timely mental health care for the 2019 novel coronavirus outbreak is urgently needed. The Lancet Psychiatry7(3), 228-229. DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30046-8.