Barban, F., Carlesimo, G. A., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., & Costa, A. (2014). Functional interplay between stimulus-oriented and stimulus-independent attending during a prospective memory task. Neuropsychologia, 53, 203–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.001
Barban, F., Scalici, F., Carlesimo, G. A., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., & Costa, A. (2020). Medio-lateral functional dissociation of the rostral prefrontal cortex with focal/non-focal cues during a prospective memory task. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(4), 1175–1186. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00063-1
Beck, S. M., Ruge, H., Walser, M., & Goschke, T. (2014). The functional neuroanatomy of spontaneous retrieval and strategic monitoring of delayed intentions. Neuropsychologia, 52, 37–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.020
Bisiacchi, P. S., Cona, G., Schiff, S., & Basso, D. (2011). Modulation of a fronto-parietal network in event-based prospective memory: an rTMS study. Neuropsychologia, 49(8),2225–2232.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.05.007
Brandimonte, M., Ferrante, D., Feresin, C., & Delbello, R. (2001). Dissociating prospective memory from vigilance processes? Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental, ISSN 0211-2159, Vol. 22, Nº 1, 2001, pags. 97-114, 22.
Burgess, P. W., Quayle, A., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 545–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(00)00149-4
Burgess, P. W., Scott, S. K., & Frith, C. D. (2003). The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 41(8), 906–918. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00327-5
Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 290–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004
Burgess, P. W., Gonen-Yaacovi, G., & Volle, E. (2011). Functional neuroimaging studies of prospective memory: what have we learnt so far? Neuropsychologia, 49(8), 2246–2257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.014
Byrne, P., Becker, S., & Burgess, N. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychological review, 114(2), 340–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.340
Ciaramelli, E., Grady, C. L., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1828–1851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022
Cohen, A.L., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Number of cues influences the cost of remembering to remember. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 149–156. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.1.149
Cohen, A.L., Gordon, A., Jaudas, A., Hefer, C., & Dreisbach, G. (2017). Let it go: The flexible engagement and disengagement of monitoring processes in a non-focal prospective memory task. Psychological research, 81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0744-7
Cona, G., Bisiacchi, P. S., & Moscovitch, M. (2014). The Effects of Focal and Nonfocal Cues on the Neural Correlates of Prospective Memory: Insights From ERPs. Cerebral Cortex, 24(10), 2630–2646. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht116
Cona, G., Scarpazza, C., Sartori, G., Moscovitch, M., & Bisiacchi, P. S. (2015). Neural bases of prospective memory: a meta-analysis and the "Attention to Delayed Intention" (AtoDI) model. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, 52, 21–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.007
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
Den Ouden, H. E. M. den, Frith, U., Frith, C., & Blakemore, S.J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. NeuroImage, 28(4), 787–796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.001
Einstein, G. O., Holland, L. J., McDaniel, M. A., & Guynn, M. J. (1992). Age-related deficits in prospective memory: The influence of task complexity. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 471–478. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.471
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R., Mayfield, S., Shank, H., Morrisette, N., & Breneiser, J. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of experimental psychology. General, 134(3), 327–342. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.134.3.327
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Prospective memory and what cos ts do not reveal about retrieval processes: A commentary on Smith, Hunt, McVay, and McConnell (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1082–1088. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019184
Eschen, A., Freeman, J., Dietrich, T., Martin, M., Ellis, J., Martin, E., & Kliegel, M. (2007). Motor brain regions are involved in the encoding of delayed intentions: A fMRI study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(3), 259–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.005
Friston, K. J., Glaser, D. E., Henson, R. N. A., Kiebel, S., Phillips, C., & Ashburner, J. (2002). Classical and Bayesian Inference in Neuroimaging: Applications. NeuroImage, 16(2), 484–512. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1091
Gilbert, S. J., Dumontheil, I., Simons, J. S., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Comment on "Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought". Science (New York, N.Y.), 317(5834), 43. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.317.5834.43
Gilbert, S. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., Cohen, A.-L., Oettingen, G., & Burgess, P. W. (2009). Separable brain systems supporting cued versus self-initiated realization of delayed intentions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(4), 905–915. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015535
Gonneaud, J., Rauchs, G., Groussard, M., Landeau, B., Mézenge, F., de La Sayette, V., Eustache, F. and Desgranges, B. (2014), How do we process event-based and time-based intentions in the brain? an fMRI study of prospective memory in healthy individuals. Hum. Brain Mapp., 35: 3066-3082. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22385
Hampson, M., Driesen, N. R., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Constable, R. T. (2006). Brain Connectivity Related to Working Memory Performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(51), 13338–13343. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-06.2006
Hashimoto, T., Umeda, S., & Kojima, S. (2011). Neural substrates of implicit cueing effect on prospective memory. NeuroImage, 54(1), 645–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.047
Hering, A., Wild-Wall, N., Falkenstein, M., Gajewski, P. D., Zinke, K., Altgassen, M., & Kliegel, M. (2020). Beyond prospective memory retrieval: Encoding and remembering of intentions across the lifespan. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 147, 44–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.11.003
Hicks, J. L., Franks, B. A., & Spitler, S. N. (2017). Prior Task Experience and Comparable Stimulus Exposure Nullify Focal and Non-focal Prospective Memory Retrieval Differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(10), 1997–2006. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1217891
Kalpouzos G, Eriksson J, Sjölie D, Molin J, Nyberg L (2010) Neurocognitive Systems Related to Real-World Prospective Memory. PLOS ONE 5(10): e13304. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013304
Kidder, D. P., Park, D. C., Hertzog, C., & Morrell, R. W. (1997). Prospective memory and aging: The effects of working memory and prospective memory task load. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 4(2), 93–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825589708256639
Kuhlmann, B. G., & Rummel, J. (2014). Context-specific prospective-memory processing: evidence for flexible attention allocation adjustments after intention encoding. Memory & cognition, 42(6), 943–949. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0405-2
Matos, P., Pereira, D. R., Albuquerque, P. B., & Santos, F. H. (2020). How Does Performing Demanding Activities Influence Prospective Memory? A Systematic Review. Advances in cognitive psychology, 16(3), 268–290. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0302-0
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), S127–S144. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.775
McDaniel, M. A., Lamontagne, P., Beck, S. M., Scullin, M. K., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Dissociable neural routes to successful prospective memory. Psychological science, 24(9), 1791–1800. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613481233
McDaniel, M. A., Umanath, S., Einstein, G. O., & Waldum, E. R. (2015). Dual pathways to prospective remembering. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00392
Momennejad, I., & Haynes, J. D. (2012). Human anterior prefrontal cortex encodes the 'what' and 'when' of future intentions. NeuroImage, 61(1), 139–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.079
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Yamadori, A., Kawashima, R., Tsukiura, T., Fukatsu, R., Suzuki, K., Ito, M., & Fukuda, H. (1998). Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: evidence from a PET study in humans. Neuroscience letters, 253(2), 127–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3940(98)00628-4
Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Yamadori, A., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2007). Differential involvement of regions of rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(3), 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.009
Okuda, J., Gilbert, S. J., Burgess, P. W., Frith, C. D., & Simons, J. S. (2011). Looking to the future: Automatic regulation of attention between current performance and future plans. Neuropsychologia, 49(8), 2258–2271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.005
Pearson, J. M., Heilbronner, S. R., Barack, D. L., Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Posterior cingulate cortex: Adapting behavior to a changing world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 143–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.002
Poppenk, J., Moscovitch, M., McIntosh, A. R., Ozcelik, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2010). Encoding the future: Successful processing of intentions engages predictive brain networks. NeuroImage, 49(1), 905–913. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.049
Reynolds, J. R., West, R., & Braver, T. (2009). Distinct neural circuits support transient and sustained processes in prospective memory and working memory. Cerebral cortex (New York, N.Y. : 1991), 19(5), 1208–1221. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn164
Rusted, J., Ruest, T., & Gray, M. A. (2011). Acute effects of nicotine administration during prospective memory, an event related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 49(9), 2362–2368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.009
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., Shelton, J. T., & Lee, J. H. (2010). Focal/non-focal cue effects in prospective memory: Monitoring difficulty or different retrieval processes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 736–749. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018971
Seibert, T. M., Gimbel, S. I., Hagler, D. J., Jr, & Brewer, J. B. (2011). Parietal activity in episodic retrieval measured by fMRI and MEG. NeuroImage, 55(2), 788–793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.078
Simons, J. S., Schölvinck, M. L., Gilbert, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Burgess, P. W. (2006). Differential components of prospective memory?: Evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 44(8), 1388–1397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.005
Smith, R. E. (2003). The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: Investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 347–361. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.347
Smith, R. E., Hunt, R. R., McVay, J. C., & McConnell, M. D. (2007). The cost of event-based prospective memory: Salient target events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(4), 734–746. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.734
Strickland, L., Heathcote, A., Remington, R. W., & Loft, S. (2017). Accumulating evidence about what prospective memory costs actually reveal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(10), 1616–1629. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000400
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549-586). Cambridge: MIT Press.
West, R., & Bowry, R. (2005). Effects of aging and working memory demands on prospective memory. Psychophysiology, 42(6), 698–712. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00361.x
West, R., Bowry, R., & Krompinger, J. (2006). The effects of working memory demands on the neural correlates of prospective memory. Neuropsychologia, 44(2), 197–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.003
Worsley, K. J., Marrett, S., Neelin, P., Vandal, A. C., Friston, K. J., & Evans, A. C. (1996). A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Human brain mapping, 4(1), 58–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O