Study selection
A total of 6,687 studies were identified through the search strategy up to December 2021, of which 6,285 were from databases and registries, and 402 from gray literature. Of the studies from databases and registries, 42 duplicates were excluded, leaving 6,243 scanned articles, of which 5,980 were excluded by title and 244 were not selected for full reading. Nineteen articles were accessed for eligibility, of which 13 were excluded and six were included in the SR. Of the 402 studies screened from websites and bibliographic citations, 10 were selected for abstract review and, of these, two were accessed for eligibility and one was included in the SR. Therefore, seven studies were included in the SR(Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012) (Fig. 1).
Study characterization
The seven studies included (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012) totaled 1,908 patients (intervention: 966, control: 942). The mean follow-up time was 4.2 years. Table 4 of the Online Resource presents the clinical characteristics of the patients of the studies by intervention and control groups. Two studies included only women (Claesson et al., 2005; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009) and the others had mostly men (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012). The mean age ranged from 53.4 to 61.5 years.
Assessment of risk of bias
Risk of bias was assessed for the primary outcome MACE and included six studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012). These studies were assessed regarding the attribution to the intervention (the “intention-to-treat” effect) and the bias due to the randomization process was classified as low in the six studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012). The bias due to intervention deviations was classified as low in two studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016) and high in four (Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012) because of the impossibility of patient blinding due to the psychological intervention. Bias due to lack of outcome data was low in two studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016) and high in four studies (Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012), considering that the missing outcome data because of losses were not similarly distributed between groups. Risk of bias for outcome measurement was rated as low in the six studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012); for selection of the reported result, it was low in five studies (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Rakowska, 2015; Schneider et al., 2012) and classified as some concern in one (Claesson et al., 2005), in which outcome analysis was performed per protocol. In general, the RoB2 characterized the assessment of the quality of the studies in this SR as presenting high risk (66.67%) and low risk (33.33%). The assessment of the risk of bias of the studies is summarized in Fig. 2.
Stress management intervention and control group
Table 5 of the Online Resource presents data on stress management interventions for each study selected. In four studies, the stress management intervention used psychoeducation that included themes such as traditional risk factors, cardiovascular system, stress, and the relationship among these components, also seeking to promote self-care and adherence to clinical guidelines (Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009). Cognitive-behavioral therapy was the most used, aiming at reducing demands and stressors, developing management skills, and coping with stress and cognitive restructuring (Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009). Two studies (Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011) used the same standardized Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy Program (Burell & Granlund, 2002) comprising education, self-monitoring, skill training, cognitive restructuring, and spiritual development, emphasizing stress management and coping to reduce the experience of daily stress and negative affectivity, with a focus on hostility and search for emotional and spiritual balance.
One study (Rakowska, 2015) used the brief strategic therapy, which identified the problems that generated stress while avoiding ineffective solutions responsible for maintaining this problem by adopting an opposite behavior. Group therapy focused on reducing stressors that cause exhaustion and on supporting recovery was used in one study (Appels et al., 2005). Another study (Schneider et al., 2012) used transcendental meditation as a mind–body intervention because of its influence on the physiological effects of stress and cardiovascular outcomes, considering its standardization, reproducibility, and validity. Muscle relaxation techniques (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009), incentive to rest (Appels et al., 2005), breathing training, use of images (Blumenthal et al., 2016), self-monitoring diaries (Claesson et al., 2005), and homework assignments (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Gulliksson et al., 2011) were also employed.
As for intervention modalities, most studies used group interventions (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009) with four–nine patients—one of them encouraging familial follow-up(Appels et al., 2005). In one study, the groups were separated by sex (Gulliksson et al., 2011) and in two studies this separation was not necessary because they comprised only women (Claesson et al., 2005; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009). Of the selected studies, only one performed the intervention individually (Rakowska, 2015). The modality of individual intervention associated with group intervention was used in one study (Schneider et al., 2012).
A for the number and duration of sessions, one study conducted 14 two-hour sessions (Appels et al., 2005), two studies conducted 20 two-hour sessions (Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011), and another had the same number of sessions but lasting 2–2.5 hours (Orth-Gomér et al., 2009). The study that used transcendental meditation taught the technique in a seven-step course comprising six meetings of 1.5–2 hours. Thereafter, follow-up and maintenance meetings were held weekly in the first month, biweekly in the following two months, and monthly thereafter (Schneider et al., 2012). Finally, one study held 10 one-hour sessions (Rakowska, 2015) and another held 12 sessions of 1.5 hours each (Blumenthal et al., 2016).
In the control group, six of the seven studies performed usual clinical treatment, which included routine cardiologist visits, clinical heart disease treatment, and traditional risk factor optimization (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009; Rakowska, 2015). In two studies, the researchers chose to refer control group patients to conventional cardiac rehabilitation (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016). In one study (Schneider et al., 2012), the control intervention was a cardiovascular health education program designed to match the experimental intervention format with instructional time, instructor attention, participant expectancy, social support, and other nonspecific factors.
As for stress measurement, 12 different assessment scales were used. The Maastricht Questionnaire was the most used (Appels et al., 2005; Claesson et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011), followed by the Everyday Life Stress Scale (Claesson et al., 2005; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009), and the Perceived Stress Scale (Blumenthal et al., 2016; Rakowska, 2015). Four studies used a single scale (Appels et al., 2005; Gulliksson et al., 2011; Orth-Gomér et al., 2009; Rakowska, 2015) and three studies assessed stress through combined scales (Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Schneider et al., 2012). In four studies, the scales were reapplied after the end of the intervention (Appels et al., 2005; Blumenthal et al., 2016; Claesson et al., 2005; Rakowska, 2015), although in one of these studies (Appels et al., 2005) the stressed patients were categorized by exhaustion level and post-intervention scale values were not directly reported.
Stress management intervention effect on cardiovascular events
In MACE analysis, no stress management intervention effect was demonstrated in reducing event risk (34.54% vs 39.05%; RR = 0.84 [CI95% 0.63–1.12], p = 0.24; PI95% 0.35–2.02; I2 = 74.7%, p = 0.001) (Fig. 3).
Sensitivity analysis, removing the studies individually, determine no significant changes in the summarized result or in heterogeneity (Fig. 1 of the Online Resource). Meta-regression for the effect modifiers age, BMI, and sex, showed no interference in the summarized data for cardiovascular events. However, when the analysis was directed to year of publication, it showed a strong potential to explain the heterogeneity (I2 = 74.7%, p = 0.001) found for the outcome (R2 = 86.9, p = 0.003; Shapiro-Wilk normality test, p = 0.25 ) (Fig. 2 of the Online Resource).
As for the analysis of isolated events, the psychological stress management intervention showed no effect in modifying the risk of cardiovascular mortality (0.77% vs 2.15%; RR = 0.38 [CI95% 0.12–1.19], p = 0.10; PI95% 0.00–5,984.93; I2 = 21.9%, p = 0.28) (Fig. 4a), nonfatal stroke (1.57% vs 2.28%; RR = 0.69 [CI95% 0.19–2.46], p = 0.57; PI95% 0.00–2,599.58; I2 = 0.00%, p = 0.87) (Fig. 4b), nonfatal acute myocardial infarction (AMI) (8.43% vs 10.28%; RR = 0.70 [CI95% 0.35–1.40], p = 0.32; PI95% 0.11–4.28; I2 = 55.3%, p = 0.05) (Fig. 4c), revascularization (17.55% vs 15.65%; RR = 1.10 [CI95% 0.86–1.41], p = 0.44; 95% PI 0.64 to 1.90]; I2 = 0.00%, p = 0.89) (Fig. 4d), and cardiovascular hospitalization (23.27% vs 27.25%; RR = 0.82 [CI95% 0.66–1.01], p = 0.06; PI95% 0.51–1.30; I2 = 0.00%, p = 0.80) (Fig. 4e).
For the outcome nonfatal AMI (Fig. 4c), which showed moderate heterogeneity, when removing the studies individually, sensitivity analysis determined no significant change in the summarized result or heterogeneity (Fig. 3 of the Online Resource). Meta-regression for the effect modifiers age, BMI, and year of publication showed no potential to interfere in the summarized result. However, when the analysis was performed by sex, the higher number of men in the sample was a strong potential to moderate the summarized outcome for nonfatal AMI (intervention: R2 = 92.0, p = 0.016; control: R2 = 84.8, p = 0.026) (Shapiro-Wilk normality test, p = 0.063). The bubble plots are shown in Fig. 4 of the Online Resource.
Effect of stress management intervention on total mortality
The psychological stress management intervention demonstrated a decreased risk of total mortality (8.58% vs 13.62%; RR = 0.63 [CI95% 0.42–0.95], p = 0.03; PI95% 0.18–2.25; I2 = 23.8%, p = 0.27) (Fig. 5).