The network theory of psychopathology has suggested that central symptoms might be valuable therapeutic targets, due to their proposed ability to fasten the deactivation of connections between symptoms 11,15, 50–55. This is one of the core propellers of the network theory of psychopathology, that lead to its growth in recent years 8. However, evidence for this hypothesis is still scarce with studies focusing on cross-sectional networks and grounding the identification of possible therapeutic targets on the initial estimations of centrality measures 24–26, 30. Due to these inconclusive results, it has been recognized that there are changes in symptoms centrality that occur during treatment 56 and that idiographic networks might be more appropriate to identify treatment targets 41. This might have important implications for treatment personalization.
In this context, we explored the impact of deactivating symptoms in contemporaneous idiographic networks through two distinct procedures. The first is based on a single time point estimate of network centrality (normal attack), and a second procedure, where, after each symptom deactivation, centrality measures are estimated again (cascade attack). The impact of symptom deactivation was assessed through a set of network properties since it has been suggested changes in the network density might be able to differentiate between different clinical presentations 17,18. However, due to the conflicting results in previous studies regarding the association between symptoms remission and networks density 23,38 and the identified changes in the network topology 31–36 we have explored the impact of symptom deactivation in two more network properties, average path length and the number of components.
Globally, our results suggest that changes in psychopathological network structure are best achieved through degree centrality. In comparison with the most common centrality metrics in psychopathological networks (i.e., strength centrality and expected influence one-step and two-step), the deactivation of symptoms by the absolute number of connections (i.e., degree centrality) seems to have a higher impact on the network structure. A previous study using cross-sectional networks 30 also found that degree centrality was the only centrality measure that was able to produce significant changes in the network structure. However, this study 30 only found significant changes in the number of components of the networks. In turn, the present study suggests that for contemporaneous within-person networks all the three network properties examined are transformed through a degree-based attack. These results suggest that different properties might be of interest according to the nature of the network (nomothetic or idiographic). Previous research has also pointed to this need for further exploration and clarification of psychological network structure 57 and the impact of the networks’ structural properties in the selection of centrality measures 58.
With the field focusing on the strength centrality and expected influence measures to identify important symptoms in the network, it’s of relevance that neither of these measures was able to promote significant changes in the network structure. In fact, the random deactivation of symptoms revealed a significantly higher impact magnitude in the number of components than a cascade attack through expected influence one and two-step and strength centrality. Thus, if changes in a person symptomatology are identifiable by changes in the network structure, the traditional psychopathological centrality metrics do not seem able to induce significant changes. Consequently, this might explain the inconclusive results in previous studies that explored if these centrality measures were related to changes in symptomatology 24–26.
It has been suggested that all centrality measures make implicit assumptions about the network processes of node-to-node transmission and the type of trajectories followed 58,59. The case may be that common centrality metrics in psychopathological networks are not accessing the specific processes that occur in these networks or are accessing some other processes that are not related to network transformation. For example, they might be identifying emergent phenomena in the network that needs to be addressed (e.g., a very active symptom) but not phenomena related to disorder maintenance (e.g., symptoms that sustain the disorder). However, in psychological networks, the processes within the networks that generate and maintain mental disorders are still unknown. Interestingly, with a cascade attack, eigenvector centrality produced significant changes in the network structure. This might be due to its suggested proximity to the causality structure of the network 29 and might mean that this measure is tapping into a specific process in psychological networks. Understanding these processes will advance the identification of treatment targets by enabling an enhanced selection of centrality metrics.
Besides exploring which network centrality metric promoted changes in the network structure, we have also tested two types of attacks, normal and cascade. Although degree centrality had a better performance than any other measure in both attacks. In the cascade attack, the magnitude and the extension were significantly higher than in a normal attack. This suggests that might be of importance to estimate symptoms’ centrality each time before an intervention is deployed to act on the symptom with the highest degree at any given time point. The dynamic fluctuations of central symptoms during a psychotherapeutic process have been highlighted by previous studies 60 and our results suggest that assessing and intervening in which symptom is central at any given time-point might produce faster recoveries. Consequently, the estimation at a single time-point of centrality measures to establish treatment targets for intervention might not be the most effective procedure to promote changes in the network structure.
This has important implications for treatment personalization. Our results suggest that to promote more effective treatments the assessment of the central symptoms must be done each time before the intervention is done. Meaning that, in the context of idiographic networks, symptomatology needs to continuously be assessed through, for example, ecological momentary assessments 61,62 for the duration of treatment to determine, at each session, in which symptom the treatment should focus. However, this leads to another important question, do psychotherapeutic strategies and psychopharmacological treatments have the specificity needed to act on a specific central symptom at each time? And is this a negative constraint of the treatments or is it a positive consequence? The answers to these questions are still unknown but the first results seem promising 63 and network analysis has all the right tools to promote more effective and personalized treatments.
Besides this, some limitations of our study should be pointed out. First, we used only centrality measures, leaving another important concept of psychopathological networks, bridge symptoms, outside of our study. Bridge symptoms have been proposed as symptoms that connect two different disorders and that acting in these symptoms might promote a faster disintegration of a comorbidity network 48,64. Our network is a comorbidity network comprising symptoms of depression and anxiety and identifying and deactivating bridge symptoms might have led to faster disintegration of the network. Another important limitation in our work is that we assume, as has been proposed in previous studies 17,18, that network properties identify psychological states, although this hypothesis is currently lacking consistent evidence 23,65. In previous studies that focused on the idiographic network, a relationship between network density and psychopathological states was found 17,18 and our results show that there are clear changes in the network density after the deactivation of 50% of the symptoms. However, we also use two network properties rarely studied in psychopathological networks and without a clear theoretical formulation, although we think it’s important to explore these new properties, we also acknowledge that there’s a need to theoretically frame these properties.