Tanalana people hold a great diversity of WEP knowledge in common, although they also display intracultural variation in WEP’s knowledge distribution, as shown by the distinct distribution pattern regarding theoretical and practical knowledge across life stages and gender. Thus, in free listings, children and adults cited different sets of species, suggesting differentiated bodies WEP theoretical knowledge. As for gender, men cited more WEP than women. We found low intracultural variation in WEP collection and preparation methods, suggesting that all Tanalana share them. We discuss such insights in terms of learning opportunities and how these might contribute to the resilience of local knowledge systems constituting a risk-management strategy. We argue that the harsh environmental conditions of the area might shape Tanalana WEP knowledge distribution and the early acquisition of this LEK by children.
Intracultural variation through life stage and gender lenses
An important insight of this work refers to the differences in plants named between children’s and adults’ knowledge, which suggest the existence of a children’s culture. As seen in our results, children know different species and methods of WEP preparation than adults and define important WEPs differently. This finding is not new, as similar findings were observed across small-scale societies and ecological regions including two cases in Madagascar (Gallois et al., 2017; Tucker & Young, 2017; Porcher et al., 2022). Thus, our insights reinforce the idea that children hold specialised ’knowledge and -potentially- what has been called “a children’s culture” (Johanson, 2010; Gallois et al., 2017). Differentiated children’s knowledge might be explained by several mechanisms driven by sociocultural and environmental contexts. One of the currently admitted theories behind children’s specific knowledge is the importance of horizontal transmission among children – from child to child- (Reyes-García et al., 2016), which fosters the acquisition of specific knowledge not shared with adults. While vertical transmission – knowledge transfer from adult to child – is usually the most intuitive process occurring, usually from parents, some studies argue that horizontal transmission might be a predominant mechanism among children in small-scale societies, as shown among the Baka people (Hewlett & Cavalli-Sforza, 1986; Gallois et al., 2018). This transmission process is usually supported by subsistence activities that encourage prosocial behaviour like playing, foraging, and food sharing (Crittenden, 2015). Tanalana children, who regularly experience the effects of the lean season, might rely in food sharing, which supports knowledge transmission between them. This mechanism adds to other, such as the fihavanana – the set of traditional rules and norms, built on values of sharing, trust, social peace and interpersonal solidarity – which structure Tanalana society and condition their cultural learning (Richter, 2001). Here, we hypothesize that environmental conditions and food scarcity might partially explain the diversified body of knowledge held by Tanalana children. Together with the transmission process, children might also create their own knowledge about their environment (Lancy, 2010) through their growing period (Ingold, 2003). Thus, the difference in species cited might also be the result of foraging decisions made by children to collect WEP best adapted to their strength, size, and preferences (Gallois et al, 2017). This might be the case for the tuber of Hydnora esculenta, only cited by children (Andriamisaina, 2019), which usually grow a few centimetres underneath the soil and require less energy to be collected than Dioscorea tubers, buried several metres deep and weighing several kilograms depending on the species.
Regarding gender intracultural variation, in contrast to most previous studies, we found that Tanalana women display a particularly reduced theoretical body of WEP knowledge, reflected in the absence of a set of WEP species specific to women and in the low number of WEP known. We argue that this finding might be explained through the ecological context of this society and women’s mobility pattern. Across cultural groups and ecoregions, women hold extensive plant knowledge (Miara et al., 2018) and particularly knowledge of WEP growing in home gardens (Díaz-Reviriego et al., 2016b) with a high specialisation in herbaceous WEP (Voeks, 2007; Porcher et al., 2022). The reduced presence of herbaceous WEP in Tanalana women's list might be explained by the xerophytic thicket characteristics, too dry for such life forms. Indeed, the only herbaceous WEP found in the region were hyper-specialised to dry conditions with succulent leaves (Fig. 2). For example, the traditional “anana” (in Malagasy), a leafy vegetable herbaceous species, that can grow wild or cultivated, and that is consumed boiled and accompanying the staple food (rice, cassava), an unavoidable dish in Malagasy diet, are absent here. The absence of this group of plants and the almost non-existence of home gardens obliterate an important part of knowledge that Malagasy women usually master in other Malagasy regions. Moreover, women and girls' reduced mobility due to their domestic-sphere-oriented activities might also affect their knowledge of WEP. Recent work in the high plateau of Madagascar shows the importance of mobility in the knowledge differentiation pattern across life stage and gender (Porcher et al., 2022). Pastoralism among the Tanalana, as other Malagasy people, is a predominantly male activity, which promotes men's mobility. In addition, in this region, during the lean season, men travel several weeks away from the village to collect WEP and water. These seasonal trips allow men to cross different types of vegetation and thus to expand their knowledge of WEP. In contrast, women who stay at the village, only have access to the nearby WEP. For example, from our interviews and additional sources (Cirad, 2015) we know that women are actively involved in collecting water when the baobab tanks are full in the dry season. These familial baobab trees (belonging to households) can be found several kilometres away from the village and therefore imply a certain seasonal mobility of women across the xerophytic thicket. Importantly, reduced women’s knowledge about WEP might have a direct effect on their nutritional intake and, by extension, their ability to cope with food crises (Jackson et al., 2020). Further studies involving socio-cultural perspectives are needed to explore gendered knowledge transmission and women’s knowledge acquisition in this region.
It has been shown that many gender-related factors could affect women's acquisition of LEK, such as early marriage, gender inequality - school dropout syndrome, difficulties in agricultural production or migration, which could increase women's vulnerability (Noromiarilanto et al., 2016; Randriamparany & Randrianalijaona, 2022). In this line, we found variation in children's preparation skills, with girls displaying lower skills than boys. Although, it would be necessary to conduct further works to elucidate this gap, we believe that it may be due to the shyness displayed by girls while being interviewed. Therefore, our results may not be representative of girls' actual practical knowledge. Verbalization of complex ideas can be a limitation for the children during interviews - where drawing methods will be more adapted- and induce shyness feeding an existing gender-shame (Gazelle et al., 2014).
Explaining early learning and children’s knowledge
One of the main insights of our study is that Tanalana children know as much as adults, citing a similar number of WEP species than adults, while reporting different species, which has not been, to our knowledge, found in other societies yet. Indeed, regarding the theoretical knowledge (i.e., the number of WEP species known), children almost reach the maximum threshold predicted for WEP species known by adults. In other words, Tanalana children know about the same number of WEP species than adults, and Tanalana adults are not necessarily more knowledgeable than children. Our results also show that useful WEP traits (life forms and resources) are known and widely spread in the community, without variation across life stage and gender. This implies that Tanalana children do not accumulate new WEP species knowledge, or very few, during the rest of their lives and that they already have information about WEP useful traits as part of their knowledge, knowing what to look for (traits). These findings are consistent with previous studies that challenge the assumption that individuals' LEK accumulation is linear all along their lives, resulting in greater expertise or skills in older individuals (Koster et al., 2016). Here we argue that high expertise may be acquired earlier in life for a given knowledge domain. According to Schniter et al. (2021), knowledge related to wild edible or toxic plants might be one of the knowledge domains acquired the earliest during lifespan. It is also true for practical knowledge, as Tanalana children share a similar level of WEP collection skills and know almost half of the preparation method reported by adults. Knowledge about WEP is acquired through children's daily involvement in gathering and preparing WEP and does not require many tools and techniques, where most of the species being are eaten raw and collected without specific tools. These relatively low complexity of techniques might thus foster an early acquisition of skills, in contrast with other subsistence activities such as hunting, for which it has been shown that skills acquisition requires a longer period of time (Demps et al., 2012; Lew-levi et al.,2021; Koster et al., 2016 and 2020). This pattern of “early knowledge acquisition” has been also observed among Mikea children, a neighbouring ethnic group of hunter and gatherers living in similar environmental conditions (Tucker & Young, 2017). The fact that children from different ethnic groups from the same arid ecoregion but with different livelihood activities, i.e., hunter and gatherers and agro-pastoralists, hold complex subsistence skills at an early age suggests an effect of the environment on children’s knowledge and the age of their acquisition. In this region, children’s “dwelt-in world” (Ingold, 2003), or the way the children experience their environment, is particularly constraint by the harsh conditions and might imply a certain engagement vis-à-vis of resources foraging resulting in a contextual specialised and early knowledge acquisition or “enskilment” (Ingold, 2003). Considering the extreme living conditions of xerophytic thicket, quickly learning how to recognize, collect and prepare WEP is definitively crucial to limiting food risk and, by extension, to survival. According to Tucker and Young (2017) this self-learning can be enhanced in a safe environment (e.g., free from predators, venomous animals, or poisonous plants) such as the xerophytic thicket, where wrong plant identification is less risky.
Risk-management strategy as a component of Tanalana’s LEK
We identify tree main aspect of the Tanalana’s LEK prone to contribute to LEK system resilience and thus constituting a risk-management strategy for Tanalana people.
A first aspect is the nature of LEK held by Tanalan people. In the xerophytic thicket, an environment which offers limited water and food resources, Tanalana people have developed sophisticated knowledge to find and prepare them, mostly in tree species and shrubs with fruits rich in vitamin A (57% of the WEP reported) and tubers used as staple food. The Tanalana have taken advantage of the hyper-specialisation and functional diversity of plants growing in the xerophytic thicket for their needs. One of the specificities of their ecological knowledge, and one of our striking results, is the large proportion of WEPs known for providing water resources. This most likely reflects the magnitude of water stress in the region. Despite the human pressure on the xerophytic thicket (Randriamalala et al., 2019), several species particularly important for water receive specific care and are protected by traditional management rules, or ‘fady’. For example, according to our informants, the collection of the water tubers of Dioscorea bermandry, ‘baboky’, is done in a non-destructive manner to ensure sustainable collection, similarly to the yam’s protoculture done by Baka people in Cameron (Dounias, 1996). Adansonia za, ‘za’, appreciated for its fruits, is also used as a water tank (Fig. 3a.6) - being able to contain up to 800 litre- by carving the trunk without killing the tree (Cirad, 2015). In sum, it seems that WEP knowledge held by Tanalana people testifies to a long experience with a limiting environment stressing hunger and thirst which is widely spread among life stages and gender.
A second aspect is the structure of the LEK system and in particular its distribution and the age of knowledge acquisition. Indeed, we showed that while children and adults look for similar plant traits supporting similar services, they both cited different WEP species and used different criteria to define WEP as important. This utilitarian redundancy might enforce adaptive capacity and knowledge system resilience (Diaz-Reviriego et al., 2016). Blanco and Carrière’s work in Morocco (2016) shows that sharing knowledge about WEP might be an adaptation to the harsh conditions of desert life - scarce and unpredictable precipitation directly affecting the food system. In this work, authors argue that the reduced knowledge variation might allow the whole community to collect most of the plant and maximise resource collection. Our results support the argument that, in a dry environment where resources are limited, most of the theoretical and practical knowledge are shared by everyone. Indeed, the observed “shared” distribution of WEP knowledge of Tanalana people (including early knowledge acquisition of children) might also be an adaptation to the difficult and unpredictable environmental conditions. In addition, Tucker and Young (2017) argue that the early skills of children advance the age of “positive net production” and thus reduce the physical and economic burdens of adults. While we did not collect any data on foraging time allocation or foraging productivity, our field observations suggest a certain autonomy of children towards WEP collection and consumption. In this sense, even if we cannot prove that Tanalana children's WEP’s foraging proficiency reduces adults’ burdens, we can suggest that it might at least improve children’s nutritional status, by providing micronutrients through daily snacking habits, as it has been shown elsewhere (Golden et al., 2011).
A third aspect that explains Tanalana’s knowledge resilience is the symbolic values and the behaviour toward WEP. If basic needs might play an important role in the WEP knowledge distribution, other socio-cultural factors are not negligible. The optimal foraging theory states that foraging is driven by the balance between energy obtained and energy spent to collect or prepare the food (Winterhalder & Alden Smith, 1981). Targeting tree species bearing fruits, which do not need specific preparation (for most of them eaten raw, see Fig. 4c P1) and providing a high energy rate due to their sugar and micronutrients, aligns with this theory of effort-gain optimization. However, several studies have shown that the cost of the gathering is not always a limiting factor and that low-energy resources are often collected despite their high cost (Hagino & Yamauchi, 2016; Gallois & Henry, 2021) challenging this neoclassical approach. The act of foraging itself, in some cases, might be perceived locally and culturally as more important than the resource provided, particularly when it relies on a collective activity because it supports socio-cultural interactions, knowledge transfer, cultural learning process and group cohesion (Gallois & Duda, 2016). Indeed, diet behaviour, including food selection and collection, depends on many psycho-cultural factors (Gaoue et al., 2017; Gallois & Henry, 2021). As Trémolière (1962) put it “human is a consumer of symbols as much as of food”. In the Malagasy culture, the symbolic act of sharing is more important when it comes to sharing a sweet dish such as fruits. In Malagasy, the word "mamy" has a double meaning, referring to both sugar/sweet and happiness. By analogy, to share sweetness, it is also to share happiness, but above all, a "sharing" and the expression of the fihavanana, the feeling of solidarity; in sum, to make a group (Ravololomanga, 2006). Thus, considering sweet WEP as important WEP is not only revealing a nutritional need but a more complex food representation -learnt at an early age- involving social behaviour such as solidarity and cooperation, enhancing the group's ability to overcome crises, though a social risk-management strategy (Moritz et al., 2011).
However, if these strategies might enhance knowledge system resilience, it does not mean that it fully protects people from food crises. Indeed, despite the great WEP knowledge held by the Tanalana people, malnutrition is still a critical challenge in this region (WFP 2020), questioning the ability of the local knowledge system to overcome crises. Recent work has stated that indigenous food system vulnerability to disaster might be approached emphasising the socio-ecological system framework while addressing several aspects of this system such as the historical, human, economic, environmental and governance dimensions (Jackson, 2020). Indeed, according to historical sources and local experiences (Ralaingita et al., 2022), despite the current food crisis with a debated climatic origin, the region experienced several episodes of intense drought followed by food crises in the 1920 and 1930. Furthermore, the 200 years of French colonisation ended in 1960 has also deeply affected the local knowledge system's integrity and thus its resilience (Raschke & Cheema, 2008). In addition, migration, poverty, the decrease in WEP abundance due to overexploitation (Schunko et al., 2022) and vegetation loss (Randriamalala et al., 2019) as well as bad governance might deeply affect the current vulnerability of the local food system. Thus, knowledge distribution is an important dimension to understanding indigenous local knowledge and food system vulnerability but to be analysed in a broader context.