With an estimated prevalence of up to 40%, chronic pain, i.e. a pain lasting for more than 3 months, is regarded as a major health problem with approximately direct and indirect costs up to 5% of the gross national product in western European countries [1]. Only in Europe, nearly one in five individuals reports having experienced moderate to severe pain at least twice a week in the previous month [2]. The fact that the number of people suffering from a chronic pain condition is steadily increasing [3], in spite of the overall improving standards of health care, emphasizes the urgent need for novel insights informing better diagnosis, prevention and treatment of patients with chronic pain.
Chronic pain markedly decrease individuals’ health status and quality of life (QoL) [4] and is linked with a wide range of physical and mental problems. Examples include as depression, anxiety and sleep disorders, and alcohol or substance abuse, either as antecedent conditions or as consequences of the development of pain [5], whereas depression and anxiety disorders are the two most frequent mental health conditions associated with chronic pain [6–8]. Furthermore, comorbid depression or anxiety is a predictor for poorer treatment response, prognosis, and social functioning, as well as higher levels of functional limitations and disability [7, 9, 10].
Several forms of chronic pain conditions have no clearly identified biological origins. In order to support the development of individualized pain management plans and to create a more accurate description of chronic pain conditions for clinicians and researchers, the 11th edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) has introduced the category of primary chronic pain, based on the terminology of the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) [11, 12]. Because primary chronic pain conditions are characterized by significant emotional distress or functional disability within a multifactorial biopsychological etiological framework and independent of identified biological or psychological contributors [12], the investigation of affective processes and mental health conditions in these disorders might yield a better understanding of the psychological processes involved in these conditions.
Self-reporting of pain is crucial for pain management. Physicians often need to know the location, intensity and quality of the pain, its impact on the patient’s activity, psychological functioning, and life in a structured format, as well as the course of the pain levels throughout the inter-visit time [13, 14]. Ecological momentary assessment techniques (EMA) are a well validated method for investigating individuals’ real-time experiences as they occur in their natural environment and situations over time, reducing memory bias [15]. It marked a notable advance in the measurement of the individuals’ every-day-pain-experience by providing data largely inaccessible to standard laboratory protocols as well as to characterize complex within-person processes over time [15–17]. EMA can be implemented to assess self-reports of pain in daily life with the help of pain diaries in digital or paper form [13, 17]. EMA has been used very infrequently to construct outcome measures in chronic pain intervention research [18], which is surprising in the light of the potential of EMA to provide ecologically valid estimates of treatment effects. EMA has been recommended for outcome measures in clinical trials to measure changes in actual pain experiences that are not confounded with symptom recollections and enhance understanding of clinical outcomes [19].
Because of the frequent association between chronic pain and affective disorders, several previous studies have used the EMA methods to investigate the association between pain intensity and affects in daily life in different samples of individuals suffering from chronic pain. Some of them focused on specific positive or negative affects, for instance, showing a significant effect of behavioral anger expression on increased pain intensity in patients with chronic lower back pain in the subsequent assessment period [20], or reporting that the worst pain experienced in everyday life was, among others, predicted by less frequently reported positive affect, such as happiness in older participants with HIV infection [21]. Although interesting, a systematic review of studies using EMA in chronic pain reported that out of 62 included articles, only 16 investigated the relationship between affect and pain intensity [18]. Relevant to the present research, several of these studies indicated a relationship between stress and self-reported pain intensity in patients with fibromyalgia [22] and lower back pain [23]. Moreover, emotional distress was shown to affect pain reports in temporomandibular muscle and joint disorders [24], with mood and stress assuming predictive roles on jaw pain in temporomandibular muscle and joint disorders [25]. Extending these findings, a study on patients with rheumatoid arthritis reported that positive mood in everyday life predicted less momentary pain, while more negative mood in everyday life predicted more pain-related restrictions intensity and depressive symptomatology predicted more momentary pain and more restrictions [26]. Similarly, a recent study combining the data of 3 EMA studies in various samples suffering from rheumatological chronic pain reported that higher levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms were observed in participants whose pain intensity reports were more strongly associated with self-reported affects in everyday life [7].
Taken together, these studies suggest a relationship between affects and pain intensity in everyday life, with the largest part of the studies investigating the relationship between stress and depressive symptomatology, suggesting that both can exacerbate the intensity of pain in daily life. To our knowledge, few studies have investigated the role of pleasant versus unpleasant affects in daily life on pain reports, and this was performed in specific samples only (for instance, rheumatoid arthritis [7]). With the introduction of the new category of chronic primary pain in the ICD-11 [12], the investigation of potential differences between patients with primary and secondary chronic pain with regard to the relationship between pain intensity and affects in everyday life might bring a better understanding of pain perception in patients with primary pain conditions. In that context, it might be interesting to also explore the role of anxiety on the reported pain experiences of individuals with primary pain conditions in daily life. In this study, we asked individuals with chronic pain (primary vs secondary) to report their experienced pain intensity and momentary affect four different times a day, for two consecutive weeks. We expected higher pain intensity in individuals with primary compared to secondary pain. We also expected higher pain intensity in association with negative vs positive affect, which would be stronger for primary vs secondary pain. Because depressive and anxiety scores were available, we explored the relationship between pain intensity with these variables.