The findings of the present study indicated that the incidence of SMI among the enrolled elderly participants was 63.9%, which is significantly higher than the rate (22%) reported by a previous study that enrolled community-dwelling elderly people in Korea [37] and Tobiansky R's findings (25%). However, it was lower than the prevalence of SMI reported in another study conducting in registrants aged above 65 years old in two primary care services in south London (66.7%) [9].This variation may result from the difference in screening tools of SMI. Kim and associates adopted the Geriatric Mental State Scale to assess memory function, while Tobiansky and associated used a semi-structured interview called short-CARE. A single subjective question asking the elderly whether they felt memory decline and the MMSE were used to screen for SMI in the present study. However, the incidence of SMI in the present study was lower than that (70%) reported by another study [33], in which the respondents were from rural areas with lower education and economic status levels. The findings of this earlier study suggested that SMI is very common among elderly individuals. Overall, the various incidence rates of SMI in the literature may be attributed to the heterogeneity of respondents from different regions with specific social and cultural backgrounds and the difference in screening tools used. Community health care personnel should pay more attention to elderly individuals with SMI, adopt more sensitive screening tools for early screening, enhance the regularity of assessments to identify memory changes, and provide memory or cognitive training as early as possible to protect the cognitive function of the elderly.
The results of the current study revealed that elderly people with SMI tend to have certain demographic factors, including older age, female sex, an unmarried status, and a lower economic status (monthly income), which are consistent with the findings of previous studies [7, 38, 39]. This study also found that elderly individuals with a high level of education (secondary or above) were more likely to report SMI, while previous studies have suggested that high education is a protective factor of memory in the elderly [40–41]. Whereas there was also evidence that the risk of Alzheimer's disease associated with subjective memory complaints is higher among those with higher education than those with lower education [42]. Elderly individuals with greater education levels tend to access to obtain more health knowledge [43], who are more aware of their health problems and sensitive to memory impairments, and thus are more likely to provide external feedback on their memory impairment problems. Especially in highly educated individuals, who still perform well on formal cognitive tests, subjective memory impairment may be an important first sign of impending Alzheimer's disease [42]. Therefore, we need to pay attention to whether older people with high level of education have complaints or behaviors regarding complaints of memory problems, and further develop more accurate screen tools for early identification of SMI and avoid the under detection of SMI.
This study also found that midday napping duration is one of the important factors influencing the occurrence of SMI. Short naps (0–30 min) and extended-length naps (> 90 min) are greater risk factors for SMI compared to moderate-length naps (31–90 min). Previous studies have also found that moderate-length naps benefit memory and cognitive function in the elderly [44]. Blackwell and associates further indicated that the longer the midday napping duration is, the higher the risks of memory decline and cognitive impairment [17]. Extended-length naps affect the occurrence of SMI in the elderly by influencing the sleep/wake circadian rhythm and disrupting nighttime sleep, leading to impairments in memory and cognitive function [45]. Midday napping is a low-cost strategy that can easily be extended to a large population as a means of potentially protecting memory function. Community health care personnel should actively give professional advice to elderly people who do not nap or who take short or extended-length naps to help them establish the better habit of taking moderate-length naps.
We further found that depressive symptoms are another important factor influencing the occurrence of SMI in the elderly, which is supported by previous studies [36, 46, 47, 44]. For example, a cross-sectional study of community adults indicated that the occurrence of SMI was strongly associated with depressive symptoms [48]. Another study further revealed that higher levels of depression were independently associated with more severe memory impairment [49]. Several mechanisms can explain the associations between depressive symptoms and SMI. First, elderly people with depressive symptoms tend to evaluate their memory performance negatively and to amplify their memory impairment [50]. Second, patients with depression have higher levels of inflammatory cytokines, and elevated inflammatory markers are associated with poorer memory and cognitive function [51–52]. Most mechanistic studies have mostly focused on groups with MCI; however, SMI is a precursor state of MCI, and more studies focusing on the mechanism by which depressive symptoms relate to SMI are warranted.
It is worth emphasizing that short naps, extended-length naps, and depressive symptoms had positive additive interaction effects, respectively, on the risk of SMI in the elderly. Notably, the interaction effect between short naps and depressive symptoms was greater than that between extended-length naps and depressive symptoms. Our results were consistent with previous studies [34, 45], which showed that extended-length naps and short naps are more detrimental than moderate-length naps to memory and cognitive function in elderly people. Wang and associates found that subjects who do not nap had higher risks for memory and cognitive impairments than those who take moderate-length naps [44]. More than 80% (3119, 82%) of subjects never take midday naps in the current study. It has been reported that insufficient napping duration is more likely to promote neuroinflammation and nerve destruction, particularly in the hippocampus, a key neuroanatomical region of memory, leading to neurodegenerative degeneration [53]. However, the mechanisms of the interaction effect between different napping duration and depressive symptoms on the occurrence of SMI in the elderly have not yet been determined. In the future, more empirical studies are needed to explore the dose–effect relationship between midday napping and SMI. Community health professionals could develop effective interventions to relieve depressive symptoms and help patients form regular midday napping habits; for example, in a previous study, a homemade gravity blanket was provided to depressive disorder co-morbid insomnia patients to influence adjustments to their nap time in order to reduce depressive symptoms [54].
This study also found that physical activity is an important factor influencing the occurrence of SMI among the elderly. In this study, elderly individuals who engage in medium- to high-level intensity physical activity tended to have better memories, which is supported by previous studies indicating that the amount of total physical activity is the main factor affecting the cognitive function of the elderly [55–57]. Another study further indicated that moderate or above physical activity for more than 90 min during the week slowed the rate of cognitive decline in older adults and contributed to the improvement of cognitive function [58]. The mechanism by which physical activity benefits memory and cognitive function can be explained according to physiological and psychological aspects. First, physical activity improves cerebral blood flow and cerebral perfusion and thus has a positive effect on brain memory function [59]. Second, people who engage in outdoor group physical activities tend to have more social links and social roles, which increase their sources of social support and reduce social isolation.
There are several improvements that can be made to this study. First, this investigation used CHARLS cross-sectional data, making it difficult to infer a cause-and-effect relationship between SMI, depressive symptoms, and midday napping duration. Future longitudinal studies could help to establish the temporal relationship between SMI and the main influencing factors. Second, the indicator of midday napping duration used in this study originated from the subjective judgments of the respondents, which may have been misreported and could lead to some information bias in the study. Future research could employ tools to record objective nap times. Finally, future studies should do cohorts to explore causal relationships between midday napping duration, depression and SMI.