Impact of writing conferences on students’ writing scores
The findings of the study revealed that the intervention and the teacher-student individual writing conferences positively impacted the students’ employment of conjunctions and referential markers and they enhanced their overall writing performance.
Table1. Mean Score, Standard Deviation, and Paired Sample t-test Results of the Pre- & Post-test
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
T
|
df
|
P
|
Pre-test
|
22.87
|
5.53
|
10.86
|
22
|
.000
|
Post-test
|
32.96
|
3.74
|
9
|
P< .05, two-tailed
Table 1. displays participants’ overall mean scores in the pre- and post-test and the results of the paired sample t-test. The mean score in the post-test was higher than it was in the pre-test. The t-test results showed that subjects’ difference in performance was statistically significant. Calculating the effect size, huge magnitude of difference was observed (Cohen’s d = 2.19).
In addition, the qualitative analysis demonstrated that the students’ post-test scripts were written more cohesively and coherently in terms of content elaboration, organization, appropriate use of referential markers and conjunctions. The remarkable improvement in the students’ post-test performance can be interpreted as the effect of the writing conferences. The individualized feedback during conferences positively impacted the students’ overall writing performance. This finding corroborates Leung’ (2008) finding that Writing Conference Group exhibit significant difference in their performance with large effect size.
Impact of writing conferences on frequency of Cohesive Devices
Table1. Mean frequency, standard deviation, and paired sample T-test results for conjunctions and references
Cohesive devices
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
t
|
df
|
P
|
Conjunctions
|
Pre-test
|
13.65
|
5.79
|
-2.200
|
22
|
.039
|
Post-test
|
15.91
|
4.33
|
References
|
Pre-test
|
29.91
|
11.01
|
-3.011
|
22
|
.006
|
Post-test
|
23.26
|
7.11
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
P<.05, two-tailed
Table 2 displays the mean frequency and the paired-sample t-test results of the conjunctions and referential markers used by the students in the guided-free writing tasks in the pre- and post-tests. As shown in the Table, the mean illustrated that there was an increase in the frequency occurrence of conjunctions in the post-test scripts. There was, however, a decrease in the frequency employment of the referential markers in the same test. The p-values of both conjunctions and referential markers indicated that difference was statistically significant. The p-values illustrate that the majority of the subjects increased their use of conjunctions and decreased their dependence on references in the post-test scripts. These findings indicated that the subjects’ post-test scripts were more densely cohesive in terms of connectedness using conjunctions than their pre-test scripts. In terms of referential markers employment, the results indicated that the subjects depended heavily on referential markers to achieve cohesion in the pre-test, whereas their dependence on referential markers diminished in the post-test.
Tables 3 and 4 further explained the contrary results, i.e. the rise in use of conjunctions and the fall in the use of referential markers.
Table3. Mean frequency, standard deviation and percent of the subcategories of conjunctions
Conjunctions
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
Percent
|
Overall conjunctions
|
Pre-test
|
13.65
|
5.78
|
8.29%
|
Post-test
|
15.91
|
4.32
|
9.93%
|
Additive
|
Pre-test
|
7.30
|
3.38
|
4.43%
|
Post-test
|
7.35
|
2.92
|
4.58%
|
Adversative
|
Pre-test
|
1.59
|
1.59
|
0.92%
|
Post-test
|
1.26
|
1.39
|
0.79%
|
Causal
|
Pre-test
|
2.41
|
1.89
|
1.40%
|
Post-test
|
4.57
|
2.25
|
2.85%
|
Temporal
|
Pre-test
|
2.64
|
2.17
|
1.53%
|
Post-test
|
2.74
|
1.45
|
1.71%
|
As demonstrated in Table 3, the increase in the use of causal conjunctions in the post-test was the most noticeable. Their frequency increased more than two times in the post-test scripts than they were in the pre-test scripts. This result indicated that the participants employed more causal conjunctions after the intervention. In the pre-test scripts, causal conjunctions ranked the third after additive and temporal respectively, whereas in the post-test scripts causal conjunctions ranked the second most commonly employed conjunction subcategory after additive conjunctions with a significant rise. The guided writing prompts in both the pre- and post-test were argumentative in nature and the subjects were required to take a position regarding the topic and to explain the reasons. The qualitative analysis showed that most of the subjects failed to explain the reasons in the pre-test scripts and their writings were obscure. Their texts lacked details and tended to be informal narrative.
Table4. Mean frequency, standard deviation, and proportion of referential markers subcategory
Referential Markers
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
Percentage
|
Overall References
|
Pre-test
|
29.91
|
11.01
|
18.16%
|
Post-test
|
23.26
|
7.11
|
14.51%
|
1st &2nd Personal Pronouns
|
Pre-test
|
11.91
|
6.11
|
7.23 %
|
Post-test
|
7.00
|
4.81
|
4.37 %
|
3rd Personal Pronouns
|
Pre-test
|
9.52
|
5.17
|
6.03 %
|
Post-test
|
11.57
|
4.97
|
7.22 %
|
Demonstrative Pronouns
|
Pre-test
|
2.39
|
1.85
|
1.52 %
|
Post-test
|
1.52
|
1.34
|
0.95 %
|
Comparative Pronouns
|
Pre-test
|
1.22
|
1.48
|
0.77 %
|
Post-test
|
0.43
|
0.66
|
0.27 %
|
The definite article ‘the’
|
Pre-test
|
5.09
|
3.04
|
3.09 %
|
Post-test
|
2.74
|
2.16
|
1.71 %
|
The zero article
|
Pre-test
|
10.61
|
5.68
|
6.44 %
|
Post-test
|
18.87
|
6.29
|
11.77 %
|
Table 4 shows that the first and second personal pronouns ranked the first most commonly employed subcategory amongst the referential markers in the pre-test scripts. It is apparent that they outnumbered the third personal pronouns in the pre-test scripts. Surprisingly, the first and second personal pronouns were even utilized at a higher frequency rate than the zero article noun phrases, which should have constituted a substantial portion of lexical cohesion in the texts. This finding revealed that the subjects depended heavily on the first and second personal pronouns than any other cohesive device rendering their pre-test scripts as vague narrative. First and second personal pronouns did not refer explicitly to a certain antecedent in the textual environment of the paragraphs. They did not contribute to the cohesion of the text according to the Halliday and Hasan’s (1976) cohesion taxonomy. Also, the abundant employment of first and second person pronouns in the pre-test scripts illustrated that the texts were informal and tended to be narrative and more of spoken discourse (Hinkel, E., 2004).
However in the post-test scripts, the frequency of the first and second person pronouns, on the one hand, and the third person pronouns on the other hand were reversed. The frequency of the third person pronouns outnumbered the frequency of the first and second person pronouns. The third person pronouns ranked the first amongst the referential markers subcategory in the post-test scripts. The first and second person pronouns frequency decreased substantially close to half. At the same time, the decrease in frequency of the first and second person pronouns was associated with a substantial increase in the frequency employment of lexical cohesive devices manifested by the rise in the frequency uses of the zero article noun phrases in the post-test scripts. They ranked as the first cohesive device followed by the third person pronouns. Further, the frequency uses of the definite article ‘the’ in the post-test scripts decreased about two times than they were in the pre-test scripts. The uses of the definite article in the pre-test scripts were most often incorrect over-use. In addition, the demonstrative pronouns frequency uses in the post-test scripts decreased to about half in the pre-test scripts. They were employed vaguely and inconsistently, but these errors diminished in the post-test scripts. These findings can be interpreted in relation to the effect of the independent variable of the study. These results demonstrated that the writing conferences positively impacted the subjects’ frequency of employing conjunctions and referential markers. They assisted the students to respond to the writing prompt properly by employing causal conjunctions to explain their position about the topic in their post-test scripts according to the nature of the writing prompt.
The decrease in mean frequency employments of the first and second person pronouns and increase in the frequency of the third person pronouns in the post-test scripts were in favor of the subjects’ performance. They depended on the third person pronouns and lexical cohesion to maintain cohesion in the post-test scripts. The use of the third person pronouns contributed to the cohesiveness of the texts by creating explicit chains of references to the antecedents within the textual environment (Hinkel, E., 2004). Hence, the students’ post-test scripts were more cohesive in terms of lexical repetition and clear chains of anaphoric references. This finding is congruent with Shaw and Liu (1998) who found that students’ texts become less like speech and more like conventional academic written English over a three-month course, though they did not use writing conferences in their study.
Impact of writing conferences on accuracy of cohesive devices
Table 5. Mean errors, standard deviations, and paired-sample T-test results of conjunctions and references
Cohesive Devices
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
t
|
df
|
P
|
Conjunctions
|
Pre-test
|
5.30
|
2.89
|
-7.253
|
22
|
.000
|
Post-test
|
0.91
|
0.90
|
References
|
Pre-test
|
12.82
|
6.19
|
-6.874
|
22
|
.000
|
Post-test
|
2.70
|
1.63
|
P< .05, two-tailed
Table 6. Frequency, proportion and ratio of correct/incorrect employment of conjunctions and references
Cohesive devices
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
Percent
|
Ratio of correct
|
Ratio of incorrect
|
Conjunctions
|
Pre-test
|
13.65
|
5.78
|
8.29%
|
61.15
|
38.85
|
Post-test
|
15.91
|
4.32
|
9.93%
|
94.26
|
5.74
|
References
|
Pre-test
|
29.91
|
11.01
|
18.16%
|
57.12
|
42.88
|
Post-test
|
23.26
|
7.11
|
14.51%
|
88.41
|
11.59
|
Table 7. Mean errors, standard deviation and paired sample t-test results of conjunctions and references in cloze-tests
Cohesive Devices
|
Test
|
M
|
SD
|
t
|
df
|
P
|
Conjunctions
|
Pre-test
|
6.82
|
1.99
|
-8.694
|
22
|
.000
|
Post-test
|
2.73
|
1.91
|
References
|
Pre-test
|
2.56
|
1.32
|
-3.016
|
22
|
.006
|
Post-test
|
1.30
|
1.99
|
P< .05, two-tailed
In Table 5., the mean errors demonstrated that there was a substantial fall in the erroneous conjunctions and referential markers in the subjects’ post-test scripts. Table 6 presents the frequency, the accuracy percentage and proportion of the employment of conjunctions and referential markers. Table 7 shows the mean errors and the paired sample t-test results of the subjects’ performance in the cloze-tests in the pre- and post-test. The p-values in both tables 6 and 7 illustrated that the difference in mean errors between the pre-test and post-test was statistically significant. These findings can be interpreted in relation to the effect of the individualized oral feedback. The writing conferences impacted the students’ accuracy employment of the conjunctions and referential markers manifested by the decrease in the subjects’ erroneous uses in post-test in both the cloze-test as well as the guided free writing tasks. This finding concurs with Aljaafreh and Lantolf (1994) who find that parrticipants appropriated the feedback and demonstrated greater responsibility over the production of the targeted structures in later sessions.
Error analysis revealed that the errors in the use of referential markers abounded excessively in the subjects’ pre-test scripts, so they hindered message intelligibility and rendered their scripts vague. The referential errors were classified into five types illustrated by the sample excerpts from the students’ scripts.
ERRORS IN THE EMPLOYMENT OF PRONOUNS:
Inconsistent use of pronouns
In the pre-test scripts, many students were unable to use pronouns consistently within and between sentences. The inconsistency occurred in three cases. The students shifted from the first person pronouns to the second person pronouns or vice versa. Second, they used pronouns inconsistently in terms of number. They shifted using a singular pronoun to refer back to a plural noun phrase or they used a plural pronoun to refer back to a singular noun phrase. As a result, antecedents of these inconsistent references were unrecoverable from the context or yielded odd ideas. Third, the students shifted from using a base form of comparison in one clause to a comparative form in the second clause within or between sentences.
- For me, I think it is very nice for me to give gifts to others because that really values your feelings towards them and sometimes they will try to give you something back as a gift.
- when you are engaged my best friend give me promes the dinner and lunch on him, but I didn’t forget his promes.
- Sometime we feel happy, but sometime [^we] feel angry. If the gift is refer to something good for you and the gift itself said thank for you. In this case we will feel happiness.
Addition of the definite article ‘the’ with generic nouns
The most frequently misused cohesive device in the students’ pre-test scripts was the definite article ‘the’. The students overused the definite article ‘the’ with generic plural countable nouns, generic non-countable nouns, and generic singular countable nouns, while they were supposed to use zero article noun phrases. Careful inspection of the students’ previous scripts before the occurrences of the definite article revealed that their uses of the definite article did not refer back to a specific antecedent within their texts. Rather, it referred to generic uses of the nouns. It seems that the students translated ideas literally from Arabic into English and unconsciously they applied the rules of generic nouns in Arabic while writing in English. This finding is congruent with Crompton (2011). Generic meaning in Arabic is expressed with generic nouns attached to the definite article 'the- ال’.
- there are a lot of things that you can do it in your society to cement the relationship with all the people…
- Some people use the gift to kill the police, as they do with some police men, somebody use the gift to joke with his friend,
- For example, the parents gave their children the gifts when they got a high level on the school. Also, the mother didn’t forget their birthdays and she appeared in the party…
Addition of pronouns
The students unnecessarily used pronouns. They, therefore, had double subjects or objects within one clause causing syntactic errors. Also, they redundantly used pronouns which were either not recoverable because they did not refer back to a specific antecedent in the previous clause or they were merely repetition.
- It’s good to give someone a gift which he like and love it.
- The presents it doesn’t matter how much money [^they] take…
- finally I think the givts [gifts] aren’t something that you receive it…
Substitution of pronouns
The students used personal pronouns to refer to too remote antecedent noun phrases instead of noun phrases. They used demonstrative pronouns to refer to a vague idea in the preceding sentences. The antecedent could be an embedded idea in the previous sentences, an antecedent noun phrase in the previous sentence or an antecedent noun phrase in the same sentence with several noun phrases in between. In addition, the students substituted vague pronouns while they could have used other obvious pronouns to convey their message effectively.
- There are so many occasions to give or receive gifts for example birthdays, widdings, turning back from traveling, and the list is endless. In addition to that sometimes we give or receive them for no any occasion.
- Some people use the gift to kill the police, as they do with some police men, somebody use the gift to joke with his friend, but sometime the joke changes to big problem. Everyone as these people must not do as this. They must use it in good ways
- I think that gift giving is something that is important in our life since it gives it a nice test (طعم). So, it is like adding salt into the food to make it delicious.
Omission of pronouns
The students omitted necessary pronouns at clause level and sentential level such as subject or object pronouns. Missing pronouns rendered their texts incorrect syntactically. The missing pronouns were written in square parentheses and marked with [^]:
- for example when they put a bad thing in a box in ceremony and give [^it] to their friend, and laughing at him.
- When you give a gift to your grandmother, grandfather, father, mother or anyone from your family [^you/it] will create very strong relationship and [^they] will love you more.
- The presents it doesn’t matter how much money [^they] take but [^they] give a good reading from others.
ERRORS IN THE EMPLOYMENT OF CONJUNCTIONS
Addition of conjunctions
The most frequently added conjunctions in the pre-test scripts were the additive conjunctions, especially 'and’ and ‘also’. The students depended heavily on coordinate conjunctions to connect sentences. They were unable to separate main ideas from supporting ideas using proper punctuation marks; so their texts replete with run-on sentences. For instance, they unnecessarily used ‘and’ in intra-sentential position at phrase level to coordinate redundant nouns or verbs. They used ‘and’ and ‘also’ in inter-sentential position at clause level to coordinate too many ideas without using a full stop (.).
- I feel very happy when someone gives me a gift and I really appreciate it so much regardless what kind of a gift it is and feel that I should give something in return to make him feel happy like what he did with me.
- I think the givts [gifts] aren’t something that you receive it no it is also some thing that you feel and a litter between your hurt [heart] you must read it correctly.
- Usually people give gifts and presents in wedding, parties, and birthdays.
Omission of conjunctions
The students were unable to use conjunctions when it was reasonable to use them in their pre-test scripts. For instance, they missed the coordinate additive conjunction ‘and’ when they were supposed to use it before the last item in a list. Also, they introduced examples to elaborate a main idea without using an exemplification transition. Similarly, when they explained cause/effect meaning relations, they did not use conjunctions to illustrate the causal meaning relations clearly. When they explained an opposite viewpoint, they did not use adversative conjunctions.
- There are many occasions for giving gifts [^ such as] birthdays, weddings, [^ and] ceremonies.
- [^When] You can give a gift to your brother or your little sister on their birthday, maybe they will crying because they couldn’t control about their emotion.
- You make all your gifts are positive. [^ For example,] I had [a story about] a negative aspect of gift giving….
Substitution of conjunctions
The students substituted erroneous conjunctions instead of appropriate conjunctions in their pre-test scripts. The substituted conjunctions were either within the same conjunction subcategory or from a different conjunction subcategory. Employing intra-subcategory substitution, the students used two conjunctions under the same subcategory interchangeably irrespective of their semantic function. For instance, within the additive subcategory they substituted ‘and’ and 'also’ interchangeably. Similarly, they substituted ‘For example’ instead of ‘such as’ at intra-sentential position in order to enumerate a list of items. Within the temporal subcategory of conjunctions, they substituted ‘finally’ as a concluding transition instead of ‘In conclusion, or In brief’ to conclude their paragraphs. Within the adversative conjunctions subcategory, the students substituted ‘By another hand’ or ‘in the other side’ instead of a normal adversative conjunction ‘However’. Further, while some transitions are fixed formulaic expressions, such as ‘on the one hand, on the contrary, therefore’ the students unwittingly created their own transition signals such as ‘in the other side/ by another hand/, for this/ for that’. With regard to inter-subcategory substitution of conjunctions, the students overused additive coordinate conjunction ‘and’ at the expense of the other subcategories. For example, while the text explained cause/effect meaning relations, the students used additive conjunctions instead.
- As it known gifts make the relationships between people stronger also people got love each other more.
- It’s good to give someone a gift which he like and love it. In the other side, somebody change the gift affect from good to bad and [^he] make a bad picture for the gift
- I think that some gifts are positive like, when you are engaged my best friend give me promes the dinner and lunch on him, but I didn’t forget his promes so for this is the most expensive gift. By another hand there are some negative gift like…