Participants
Inclusion Criteria
Inclusion criteria for all participants were as follows: (1) between 8 and 10 years of age; (2) have a score above 70 in the verbal and performance IQ on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - IV Edition (WISC-IV); and (3) have no history of brain damage, epilepsy, psychosis, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), bipolar disorders (early-onset bipolar disorder), Tourette’s syndrome, childhood depression; (4) have no current aggressive behaviour or severe oppositional tendency; (5) have no hearing, visual, or physical disabilities, and (6) not being under psychiatric medication.
Children with comorbid disorders were excluded to increase the specificity of the results and avoid confounders. More specifically, children with ODD can be uncooperative, hostile toward authority and behave with the aim of annoying others. Including children with ODD in the study could have raised the doubt that a lower score could have been due to uncooperativeness or plain opposition to the task instead of a neurocognitive deficit.
The hypothetical monetary DD task has been used extensively with adolescents and occasionally with children. However, a recent study found that the generated data suggest a disengagement or misunderstanding of the task[69]. Thus, to ensure the understanding of the task, we required all participants to show good understanding of the concepts of time and money using the Concepts of Time and Money Questionnaire (CTMQ).
Participants included in the EG also have a cut-off severity scores of 14 or higher in both ADHD-I (inattentive subgroup) and ADHD-H (hyperactive subgroups) subscales, i.e. ADHD-C criteria (combined inattentive and hyperactive subgroups) on SDAI and a clinical diagnosis of ADHD based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5)[3] criteria obtained from a licensed clinical child psychiatrist. The diagnosis was further confirmed through an additional assessment by the consensus of experienced clinicians in the research team (i.e. child psychiatrists and clinical psychologists).
Recruited Population
N= 414 Italian children, aged 8 to 10 years old, attending 4th or 5th grade of twelve public primary schools in Lombardy, Northern Italy, were recruited. Referrals for children with ADHD were obtained from professionals taking part in an ongoing research and placement training program. All contacted schools agreed to participate in the research and were located in urban areas with an average socio-economic status (SES), private schools were not included. A first screening, administered by teachers, based on the Italian versions of the ADHD Rating Scale for Teachers (SDAI)[10][39] and the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (DBDRS)[38] were used to assess ADHD traits in the participants prior to the beginning of the study.
Teachers were asked to observe the recruited children for two weeks and to complete SDAI and DBDRS for each child. Subsequently, they had to report the frequency of any problematic behaviour according to a Likert scale from 0 (problematic behaviour never presents) to 3 scores (very often present) for each questionnaire.
Thirty children were eligible for the EG according to the ADHD-C criteria on SDAI and were further assessed using CTMQ. Each participant was asked to answer 16 open-ended questions on knowledge regarding the values of money and time. Based on the results, N=22 children were eligible for the EG.
WISC-IV[52], DBDRS and the Parent Interview for children Symptoms (PICS-IV) scales were administered to the children eligible after CTMQ. These scales aimed to identify disruptive behaviour disorders or other psychiatric disorders, together with getting information about school achievement from parents, teachers, and students themselves. After this test, N=6 children were excluded because diagnosed with ODDs (figure 1).
The digit span of each of the participants was assessed before starting the experiment using the digit span forward in the WISC-IV. Assessments’ data for each participant are reported in the Supplementary Materials, sheet S1.
The selected children were individually examined by experienced professionals of the research team and parents were interviewed to confirm their authorisation to be part of the EG.
N=16 children (11M:5F; mean age in years = 8.75; SD = .48) fully met the inclusion criteria and were enrolled as EG in the present study.
In the final sample there were 14 classes from 11 schools with either 1 or 2 children for each of the twelve participating classes in each group.
The selection was performed separately for each participating school and class to ensure the best matching among social and educational exposure and experimental setting.
We used a randomized block selection among children eligible for the CG to ensure that the groups were balanced for number of children, class, and gender. N=16 children (11M:5F; mean age in years = 8.62; SD = .62) were randomly assigned to the CG.
All selected children for CG participated in the study and were assessed using the same method used for the EG. All children in the CG meet the inclusion criteria. We had no dropouts.
The EG displayed significantly higher scores than CG on both SDAI hyperactivity, t(22.4) = 38.5, p < .001, SDAI inattention subscales, t(15.0) = 70.5, p < .001, while there were no significant statistical differences in IQ, t(30) = 1.29, p = .208 and Digit Span Forward task, t(30) = .582, p = .565. Furthermore, there were no difference in time and money understanding on CTMQ: Time scale, t(28.7) = 1.17, p = .253, Money scale, t(22.6) = 1.85, p = .077.
The Control Group was comparable to the EG regarding IQ, age, year of education, Digit Span and CTMQ scores. All children in the CG showed no clinical signs of ADHD or other developmental conditions.
Demographic and assessment data for both experimental and control groups are summarised in Table 1.
Measures
The SDAI is an ADHD questionnaire widely used in Italy, validated and standardized for the Italian population[11][46]. It is composed of eighteen items matching the symptom domain of ADHD as described in the DSM-5[3]. It has a reliability of 0.80 (Inattentive subscale) and 0.74 (Hyperactive/Impulsive), optimal discriminatory power and concurrent validity (r > 0.95;[39]. Its test-–retest reliability is 0.83 and 0.81) for inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive respectively[37].
The DBDRS is a 45-question screening measure, completed by either parents or teachers, designed to identify symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and conduct disorder in children and adolescents.
The CTMQ was developed for this study and is reported in the supplementary materials. The cut-off was set to 15 correct answers among 16 questions in both time and money scales. CTMQ was based on our knowledge of children’s cognitive development and reviewed by a focus group of five primary school teachers. The majority of students are expected to pass the test and it is designed to assess the skills’ level needed to correctly understand the experimental task. CMTQ was included in the study because children with ADHD can have difficulties with magnitude-related concepts like space, time and numbers[57]. Researching the relation between those deficits it’s outside the scope of our experiment but previous studies shown a neurological link with the implication of the parietal cortex in both magnitude representation[58] and ADHD[59]. In our study, which focuses on impulsivity and decision making, possible difficulties in time and monetary value representation would be confounders.
Experimental Design
A dedicated application was developed for the administration of the individual sessions. All sessions were administered in a quiet classroom of the school, equipped with computers, monitors, chairs and desks. All participants were asked to sit in front of a monitor in order to complete the tasks. Each session consisted of one preliminary trial and 3 blocks with different memory loads with 16 experimental trials for each block comprising the random combination of 4 delay and 4 reward conditions. Each session lasted 20 minutes.
Preliminary Task
Each of the N=32 participant was asked to engage in a second individual assessment of memory span using the software developed to run the experiment instead of the WISC-IV[52]. The aim of this preliminary task was to find the baseline digit span for the participant given the different experimental condition between the experimental setting and the initial assessment.
For the preliminary assessment of maximum memory load, the first sequence was 3-digits long. The number of digits to repeat would increase if the participants successfully managed to repeat all of them. A new number sequence was randomly generated before each trial.
Memory Load Task
In the memory load task, the participant was required to listen and repeat the assigned sequence of random numbers.
Three different conditions were tested and counterbalanced among participants: (1) delayed reward option task without the memory load task; (2) dual-task with the delayed reward option task and the memory load task with half memory load and (3) dual task with full memory load.
The initial full load was set to the maximum digit span assessed in the preliminary task minus one (n-1), while the half load was set to n/2 if n was even and (n-1)/2 if n was odd.
We choose (n-1) as the full load condition to minimize the number of errors while maintaining a high load on WM. This task not only functions as an interference but it’s also a measure of the attention to the experiment. A high number of errors in this task would mean that participants are deploying their attentional resources mainly on the primary task instead of splitting them between the two tasks.
In the conditions with half and full memory load, the random number sequence was presented before the choice task, participants were therefore asked to remember the sequence and repeat it aloud through the choice task. To ensure that the participants’ cognitive resources were focused on the memory load task, if an error in repeating the digits was made, another series of the same digits was presented; if the participant failed also to repeat the second series, the task was stopped and the number of correct digits was recorded as the maximum digit span of the participant for that specific trial.
Delayed Reward Task
The primary task consisted of a series of choices between a small monetary reward obtained after a shorter time and a larger monetary reward obtained after a longer time. The delayed reward option task comprised a series of 16 trials. In each trial, the participant was asked to select between two options e.g. the first option was always a smaller amount of money the same day, whilst the second option was a larger amount of money deferred in different periods of time. Each participant knew that there were no right or wrong choices. Trials comprised 4 possible reward combinations: €1-€11, €2-€12, €3-€13 and €4-€14, with a reward difference always set to €10; and 4 possible time delays of 1 week, 1 month, 6 months and 1 year. An algorithm specifically designed for the test had selected the order of the 16 trials randomly and balanced them among participants.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were run through SPSS software (v. 23, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). The descriptive statistics of the dependent variables were examined. The dependent variable (the value of k) were submitted to repeated measure ANOVA with one between-subject factor (group), and two within-subject factors: (1) three memory load conditions (zero load, half load, full load) and (2) four time delays (1 week, 1 month, 6 months and1 year); trials within the same condition but different monetary rewards were averaged together. The alpha-level was set to .05 for all statistical tests. All effect sizes and post-hoc tests’ power are reported in sheet S3 of Supplementary Materials. The effect sizes were computed using partial eta-squared. Omnibus tests were evaluated with two-tailed alpha-level = .05. Planned pairwise comparisons were performed among groups using t-test and ANOVA, alpha-level was Sidak’s corrected. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used for effects failing the Mauchly’s test of sphericity.
With reference to the primary task parameter, the inverse formula of was calculated: . k is a parameter that measures the decrease rapidity in subjective value over time. A k value of zero shows an absolute preference for delayed choice, while higher k values show an increase in DD. In our analysis we used Euros for A and V and weeks for D (setting a month to 4 weeks) as units. The specific units of measurement that have been chosen, change the results only by a multiplicative factor, the choice is therefore irrelevant for the purposes of statistical analysis.
Preliminary ANOVA was carried out to examine the secondary task performance, assuming digit span as the dependent variable to ensure that the relevant variable was the memory load and not the memory span. Correlation among variables were calculated to assess their impact on the analysis.
Delayed discounting rate defined as DR = -ln(k) is also commonly reported in the literature, especially when researchers prefer to use logistic regression instead of the inverse formula to compute k [77].Therefore, we used it as a supplementary analysis for comparisons between groups and among different WM loads.
Ethics, consent and permissions
The study was conducted in accordance with the American Psychological Association’s (APA) ethical standards. The study design was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of Cognitive Science, Psychological, Educational and Cultural Studies of the University of Messina approved the study protocol (Protocol n. 20015_34). All the parents of the children who took part in the study signed a written consent form.