The present study adopted the task-switching paradigm to investigate whether clinically depressed patients have dysfunctions in external switching and backward inhibition and whether these dysfunctions are specific to emotional content. In line with Whitmer and Gotlib 14, switching cost and BI were calculated based on comparisons between specific trial types.
Our Results first showed that the switch cost in non-emotional tasks was significantly greater for MDD than for NDC, suggesting a general dysfunction of external switching among MDD. Switch costs also clearly demonstrated emotion-related switching dysfunction in MDD relative to NDC participants, and critically, the switching cost was greater for the emotional-related task than the non-emotional task, indicating both general and emotional-specific external switching dysfunction was found in the current study. This result was inconsistent with prior studies of Whitmer and Gotlib 14, who failed to find any non-emotional switching dysfunction in MDD except when the subjects were induced to ruminate. This result also contradicted the results of De Lissnyder, et al. 27, who failed to find any general external switching dysfunction and emotional-related dysfunction of switching in participants with moderate or severe depressive symptoms. Note that those two studies suffered from various problems that would confuse their results as analyzed in the introduction and our finding was more reliable. Further, we found that the dysfunction in MDD was mainly manifested in switching from the emotional task to the non-emotional task, rather than the reversed switching, suggesting a valence-specific dysfunction. The results that the difference between “Cost-EN” and “Cost-NN” was significant for MDD but not for NDC indicated the valence-specific dysfunction in MDD may be related to their difficulty in suppressing the emotional task when switching to a non-emotional task. The finding of valence-specific dysfunction of switching in MDD conforms theoretically to the impaired disengagement hypothesis 50, which postulates that depression is associated with impaired attentional disengagement from negative self-referent information. The results were also confirmed by the evidence from eye-movement studies, that is, MDD showed an impairment of attentional disengagement from negative emotional information. For example, Eizenman, et al. 51 found that MDD patients spent significantly more time looking at negative emotional information than did NDCs. Recently, Sanchez, et al. 52 found that MDD patients were normal and similar to NDCs in their initial orientation to negative stimuli but had specific difficulties in disengaging their attention from such stimuli.
Secondly, our results showed that the BI effect in emotional tasks was robust in NDC but not in MDD, suggesting dysfunction of emotional BI in MDD. However, when the effect of rumination was controlled, the group difference was eliminated, i.e., comparable emotional BI effects were observed in MDD and NDC. These results indicated that rumination but not depression was associated with dysfunction of the emotional BI effect in external switching among MDD patients. The relationships between rumination, switching and BI in the present study was in line with those reported by Whitmer and Gotlib 14, who found that trait rumination was also related to BI dysfunction but not to increased switch costs.
Most critically, however, the BI in the study of Whitmer and Gotlib 14 was about inhibition in non-emotional tasks, while in the present study, it was about inhibition in emotional tasks. we did not find any significant BI effect except a reversed BI effect in non-emotional tasks (-128 ms and − 27 ms, respectively, for MDD and NDC). One possible explanation is that with the sequence of trials used in measuring the non-emotional BI effect, in both the A-B-A and C-B-A sequences, “B” was an emotional task in trial N-1. Since the emotional task will automatically attract attention and processing capacity 53 in trial N-1, participants would not need to suppress information in the non-emotional task (“A” or “C”) performed in trial N-2. That is, the residual activation of the “A” task would facilitate the “A” task when the participant switched the task again for trial N, displaying a reversed BI effect.
From the magnitude of the reversed BI effect, one can also infer that the easier the attentional bias to the emotional face in trial N-1 (e.g., MDD, 54), the greater the facilitation of the same task from trial N-2 to trial N. Considering that the non-emotional BI effect was influenced by the emotional task in the present study, it remained unknown whether there is a general dysfunction of BI in MDD. Future studies could test this issue with a purely non-emotional task-switching task.
Briefly, although we failed to replicate the non-emotional BI dysfunction in Whitmer and Gotlib 17, our results extended the association between trait rumination and BI dysfunction in MDD to the emotional domain, i.e., MDD patients have an emotional dysfunction of BI, negatively associated with rumination but independent of depression.
The cause of such BI dysfunction may be the failure in the suppression of negative emotional faces. Ruminative thinking can overload limited executive resources and therefore impair task-relevant processing depending on those resources 55. Available executive resources to inhibit the previous, abandoned negative task when switching to the new task is hence reduced. In support of this possibility, multiple studies have shown that rumination is related to dysfunction in the inhibition of negative emotional information 56,57.
Another possibility is related to the “automaticity” of emotion processing. Piguet, et al. 58 found a reduction in BI effect in emotional trials with normal participants. Since emotion can facilitate perception, attention, and memory in various situations 59, and since social emotional processing (such as face processing) tends to operate automatically 41, the impact of inhibition from the preceding task on the current target will be reduced when the target is an emotional face.
This explanation is supported by the neural deactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex, which is positively related to attentional control and effort to overcome the inhibition of the preceding task during the switch to a new task. Rumination in MDD was shown to correlate with an attentional bias for negative emotional faces even after controlling for BDI scores 60. This may render the processing less susceptible to interference from inhibition of the preceding task for emotional faces than from non-emotional faces, hence eliminating the emotional BI effect for MDD.
In fact, both can be explained by the recent Attentional Scope Model of Rumination 57. According to this model, MDD patients with stronger ruminative tendencies have a narrower attentional scope; when a negative emotional face appears in the visual field, the depressed mood creates a negative processing bias, and then the narrowed attentional scope will lead MDD patients to increase cognitive resources devoted to deep encoding and activating this negative information (i.e., the “automaticity” of emotion processing). This shift in resources, in turn, increases the difficulty of inhibiting the activation when this negative information stops being relevant (i.e., failed suppression).
Thirdly, compared with the effect of rumination on BI, the results of regression in the present study did not show any effect of rumination on switch costs. That is, rumination in MDD plays a major role in the dysfunction of emotional BI, but irrelevant to the dysfunction of emotional switching. Obviously, this conclusion is consistent with that of Whitmer and Gotlib 14. Moreover, as discussed before, our results extended their conclusion to the emotional domain.
In addition, by adopting the Internal Shift Task, prior studies by Lo and Allen 15 and De Lissnyder, et al. 25 also demonstrated the dysfunction of switching in MDD. However, their results were conflicting, i.e., Lo and Allen 15 observed emotion-specific rather than general dysfunction with affective/neutral words, while De Lissnyder, et al. 25 observed general but not emotion-specific dysfunction in MDD. Although there has been no research addressing this discrepancy, we speculate that it may be related to one problem in the experiment of De Lissnyder, et al. 25. Specifically, although the task for the participants was to identify the gender of the face in the non-emotional task, the expression of the face may also be angry. Hence, MDD patients could automatically process the negative face, potentially blurring the difference between emotional and non-emotional (general) task switching. The same problem applies to the study of De Lissnyder, et al. 27. In addition, many prior studies have provided evidence to indicate the distinct cognitive functions underlying the task-switching task and the Internal Shift Task 34,35. Therefore, it is possible to obtain distinct patterns of results in MDD patients with the two paradigms.
The results of the present study have important clinical implications for improving the understanding of cognitive vulnerability factors for depression, such as impaired cognitive control. If cognitive processes are causally linked to the root problems, modifying these underlying cognitive processes holds promise as a way to improve depressive symptoms 61. Cognitive control training is a new intervention for depression that could facilitate rehabilitation and remission depression, helping improve patients’ quality of life and prevent relapse 3. Our results can help by providing a reference regarding the target of training.
The present study also has several limitations. First, as only angry faces were used here as emotional stimuli, we cannot conclude that the impairments can be generalized to other negative material (such as sad emotions). It is necessary to compare the effects of different negative stimuli in the future. Second, because the non-emotional BI effect was impacted by the emotional task, it remains unclear whether there is a general dysfunction of BI in MDD. Future studies can test this issue with a purely non-emotional task-switching task. Third, because of the limitations of the participant sample, we cannot generalize the conclusion to other depressed populations, such as the non-medicated depressed population. Fourth, the impact of several important potential factors (such as anxiety score, comorbidity, frequency and history of medication use, and age) on the results could not be excluded. Finally, the association between depression and dysfunction in external switching and BI in the present study does not provide information about causality. Future research should focus on the functional relationship between these constructs.
In conclusion, the present study provides more insight into the relationship between depression and dysfunctional cognitive control, especially in external switching and BI. The results suggested that MDD patients have both general dysfunction and emotion-specific in external switching and emotional dysfunction in BI. In addition, rumination plays a vital role in BI dysfunction but not in external switching dysfunction.