Several factors might create bias in findings. For instance, districts with higher concentrations of a particular ethnic group may have fewer economic resources, such as job opportunities and businesses, making it more difficult for residents to find employment. Another possibility is that discrimination and prejudice based on race or ethnicity may be more prevalent in neighborhoods with higher concentrations of a particular group, making it more difficult for members of that group to access employment opportunities. The complex relationship includes various factors such as economic resources, discrimination, and other social and cultural factors. Therefore, to ensure the DID results are not biased and valid, a series of descriptive analyses across all the treatment and control groups were conducted. These analyses confirmed that treatment and control groups were not significantly different before the refugee influx, and observed differences from the study are solely due to the intervention, i.e., refugee influx rather than preceding differences.
5.1.1 Descriptive statistics
Table 1
Sample characteristics and correlation with output variables
|
Observations
|
Mean
|
Correlation
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
Language
|
Refugees (countries)
|
|
|
|
|
Arab (Syria, Iraq & Afghanistan)
|
55,703
|
|
-0.16*
|
-0.07*
|
Refugees (continents)
|
|
|
|
|
Asian refugees
|
58,889
|
|
-0.14*
|
-0.06*
|
African refugees
|
4,322
|
|
0.06*
|
0.02*
|
European refugees
|
11,608
|
|
0.07*
|
0.03*
|
Employment (y/n)
|
27,333
|
0.29
|
-
|
-
|
Language adaption(y/n)
|
25,260
|
0.72
|
-
|
-
|
Female (gender)
|
79,285
|
0.45
|
-0.26*
|
-0.16*
|
Age (years)
|
78,933
|
26.79
|
-0.04*
|
-0.20*
|
House prices
|
-
|
214.37
|
0.06*
|
0.03*
|
Agricultural employment
|
-
|
0.90
|
-0.085
|
-0.01*
|
Note: The table shows descriptive statistics for the sample used in this study |
The table above provides a general overview of the study sample, with a total number of observations, the mean of observations across treatment and control groups, and their differences. Lastly, the correlation of the independent variables with the outcome variable portrays the direction and strength of the relationship between our covariates and output variables. The significant correlation for all our variables indicates that each variable has a significant bivariate relationship with the outcome variable, employment, and language adaption (see columns 3 and 4).
Table 2
Treatment and control groups comparison
|
Treatment
(1)
|
Control
(2)
|
Difference
(3)
|
Standard error
(4)
|
Arab
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
0.51
|
0.55
|
0.04
|
0.03
|
Oral language adaption
|
0.88
|
0.95
|
0.07
|
0.02
|
Asian
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
0.52
|
0.54
|
0.02
|
0.03
|
Oral language adaption
|
0.88
|
0.96
|
0.08
|
0.02
|
African
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
0.53
|
0.52
|
-0.01
|
0.03
|
Oral language adaption
|
0.88
|
0.95
|
0.06
|
0.02
|
European
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
0.52
|
0.53
|
0.01
|
0.03
|
Oral language adaption
|
0.89
|
0.93
|
0.04
|
0.02
|
Note: The average characteristics of the treatment and control and the differences before the refugee influx in 2015 are shown in columns 1 to 3, followed by the Standard Error (S.E) in column 4. The characteristics are shown for all three ethnic groups Arabs (1 to 4) Asians (5 to 8) and Europeans (9 to 12). In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level. |
The table above shows the mean differences between all the treatment and control groups before the refugee influx. Our results show no significant difference among our measures for economic and social integration, employment, and language adaption, prior to the refugee influx, among the treatment and control districts indicating the differences post-influx are due to the refugee influx and not pre-existing differences. To verify the relationship and impact of refugees being assigned to districts with existing refugee ethnic clusters, employment, and oral language adaption measure is regressed with ethnic refugee share in the general population along with other factors that might influence the outcome variables, such as years of living in the Germany, age, and gender. As the districts might differ in other characteristics, I included district-fixed effects to account for the differences. In table 3, the OLS regression indicates a relationship between ethnic clusters and refugee integration. For instance, the significant negative coefficient for Arab and Asian refugees indicates that ethnic clusters negatively influence employment. However, the analysis is for each year separately from 2013 to 2020; the results indicates no significant association between the two factors before the refugee influx in 2015. There, for our benchmark analysis, we have set the post period from the year of the refugee influx, 2015
Table 3
The relationship between ethnic cluster and refugee integration
|
Dependent variable: employment status
|
Dependent variable: Oral language adaption
|
|
Arab
(1)
|
Asian
(2)
|
African
(3)
|
European
(4)
|
Arab
(5)
|
Asian
(6)
|
African
(7)
|
European
(8)
|
Ethnic refugee share
|
10.62***
(1.22)
|
6.83***
(0.98)
|
42.44***
(14.56)
|
-15.57
(17.68)
|
14.81***
(1.20)
|
12.26***
(1.01)
|
26.95**
(12.29)
|
-44.12***
(16.81)
|
Years in Germany
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.03***
(0.004)
|
0.01***
(0.002)
|
0.04***
(0.001)
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.05***
(0.003)
|
0.02***
(0.002)
|
Age
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.003***
(0.001)
|
-0.003**
(0.001)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.001)
|
-0.01***
(0.001)
|
Gender: Female
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
-0.30***
(0.02)
|
-0.13***
(0.03)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.20***
(0.02)
|
-0.06**
(0.03)
|
Observations
|
17,631
|
19,214
|
2,349
|
1,436
|
17,019
|
18,283
|
2,240
|
1,097
|
R-squared
|
0.1979
|
0.2006
|
0.3069
|
0.3930
|
0.2043
|
0.1918
|
0.2841
|
0.3736
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows the results the of difference-in-differences method (columns 1 to 8). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measure whether the individual is employed or not. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
5.1.2 Benchmark analysis
Tables 4 and 5 reports the estimates of equations 1 to 3. In columns (1–8), the generalized DID results show the effect of ethnic clusters on refugee integration due to the 2015-16 refugee influx. Each column and the following column show results of regression with controls. All the regressions are controlled for district-fixed effect. Tables 3 and 4 captures the refugee influx effect on economic and social integration, employment, and German language adaption. The columns represent the results of DID for Arab, Asian, and European samples separately.
The first column shows the estimates of the effect of ethnic clusters on refugee integration without controlling for individual and district-level characteristics. Given that the characteristics mentioned in table 1 likely correlate with employment, we control for all these factors, and the results are shown in the following column (2). The analysis is run across different ethnic groups, Asian refugees (columns 3 and 4) and African refugees (columns 5 and 6), and European refugees (columns 7 and 8). In tables 3 and 4, the estimates compare districts with above median refugee share (treatment) and below median refugee share (control). The estimates from the table show that in treatment districts (districts with higher ethnic share), Arab refugees are 4.60% and 10.63% better at finding employment and adapting to language; however, Asian refugees are 9.27% and 11.43% less likely to find employment and adapt to the German language. Whereas, for African and European refugees, there were no significant differences in employment and language adaption.
As discussed in section 3, the current literature supports both positions regarding the relationship between ethnic clusters and refugee integration. The results of this study contribute to the literature by finding a better understanding of the relationship by illustrating how ethnic background is the decisive factor in the relationship. For instance, (i) the positive relationship between ethnic clusters and integration for war-torn Arab refugees can be related to Marten, Hainmueller, and Hangartner’s (2019) study, where higher ethnic concentration made it easier for refugees to find employment, improving living standards and reduce government’s burden on social benefits. In addition, learning a second language is easier with lower language barriers with the same ethnic teachers. (ii) The negative relationship between ethnic clusters and integration for refugees from the Asian continent is supported by Braun and Dwnger’s (2017) study, where they found segregated neighborhoods to have higher poverty levels and lower economic development, harming both the refugees and the host communities. Relating to social identity and social representation theory, an individual’s behavior or actions would be influenced by their race (i.e., group). For Arab refugees from Syria, Iraq, and Afghanistan, having more group members in a particular district improves their probability of finding jobs or local language adaption compared to areas with lower group members, portraying how collective culture among Asians and Arab works. The membership can be understood to initiate collaboration and teamwork, improving employment opportunities. For instance, a group member would refer another member or help them to find jobs. These results align with Marten, Hainmueller, and Hangartner (2019) and Fasani, Frattini, and Minale (2021). Whereas, in comparison, the negative influence of the Asian ethnic cluster can be related to several factors. First, refugees with limited German language proficiency might suffer in communicating with people outside of the ethnic cluster due to lack of interaction with the host population, harming both host language adaption capabilities, and job opportunities. Second, these ethnic enclaves inadvertently block exposure to local culture and community, harming further interaction and, therefore, successful integration. Third, the Asian refugee sample for this study has refugees from different geographical and cultural backgrounds, ranging from South Asian countries such as Bangladesh, and Pakistan to Eastern Asian countries such as the Philippines and Vietnam to Central Asian countries such as Azerbaijan and Uzbekistan. Therefore, relating to Tajfel and Turner’s (1979) formation of social groups helping to integrate might be comparatively more difficult.
On the contrary, African and European ethnic clusters showed no influence in finding employment and adapting local language. Though Africans are collectivist in nature, the hundreds of language dialects across the population might offset the influence on the formation of ‘groups’ based on language or ethnicity, which might influence integration measures. However, several other reasons might influence this relationship which the model is unable to capture, such as perception, attitude, and eagerness to integrate. The probable reason behind ethnic clusters having no influence on refugee integration for Europeans might be due to two major reasons. First, in general, Europeans are understood to be individualistic in nature, compared to the collectivist nature of Asians. Individualism in nature favors freedom of action or not being dependent like collectivist groups. Second, Europeans find it hard to differentiate and create smaller groups, as the majority are Europeans or ‘white’ in Germany’s content. Comparatively, minor groups find it easier to organize by virtue of their small size and tend to focus on a narrow set of issues (Saideman, 2022). Nonetheless, as significant literature and policymakers have emphasized ethnic clusters to negatively harm integration, I further test the findings across districts that are outliers, in terms of receiving the highest number of refugees in table 6.
Table 4
The effect of ethnic clusters on refugee employment
|
Arabs
(1)
|
+controls
(2)
|
Asians
(3)
|
+controls
(4)
|
Africans
(5)
|
+controls
(6)
|
Europeans
(7)
|
+controls
(8)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
8.50***
(3.44)
|
7.87***
(2.84)
|
30.65***
(0.11)
|
20.27***
(3.80)
|
98.33***
(29.16)
|
87.48***
(27.45)
|
-4.05
(21.22)
|
-11.78
(21.86)
|
Post influx
|
-0.44***
(0.03)
|
-0.13***
(0.04)
|
-0.24***
(0.03)
|
-0.12***
(0.03)
|
-0.41***
(0.07)
|
-0.20***
(0.073)
|
-0.06
(0.04)
|
-0.11**
(0.05)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
13.85***
(2.63)
|
4.60**
(2.24)
|
-17.27***
(3.61)
|
-9.27***
(3.35)
|
-9.05
(16.39)
|
-16.06
(15.72)
|
-2.52
(9.71)
|
7.76
(10.36)
|
Years in Germany
|
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
|
0.03***
(0.004)
|
|
0.01***
(0.002)
|
Age
|
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
|
-0.003***
(0.001)
|
|
-0.003**
(0.001)
|
Gender: Female
|
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
|
-0.30***
(0.02)
|
|
-0.13***
(0.03)
|
Observations
|
17,077
|
17,631
|
19,287
|
19,214
|
2,364
|
2,349
|
1,624
|
1,436
|
R-squared
|
0.0682
|
0.1987
|
0.0769
|
0.2026
|
0.2042
|
0.3096
|
0.3666
|
0.3962
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows results of difference-in-differences method (columns 1 to 6). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measure whether the individual is employed or not. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
Table 4
The effect of ethnic clusters on refugee German language adaption
|
Arabs
(1)
|
+controls
(2)
|
Asians
(3)
|
+controls
(4)
|
Africans
(5)
|
+controls
(6)
|
Europeans
(7)
|
+controls
(8)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
8.50**
(3.44)
|
4.03
(3.09)
|
40.95***
(4.16)
|
27.28***
(3.89)
|
51.23**
(27.58)
|
28.70
(24.96)
|
-44.04*
(24.51)
|
-38.60*
(23.15)
|
Post influx
|
-0.44***
(0.03)
|
-0.07**
(0.04)
|
-0.25***
(0.03)
|
-0.09***
(0.03)
|
-0.37***
(0.07)
|
-0.06
(0.06)
|
-0.01
(0.05)
|
-0.03
(0.05)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
13.85***
(2.63)
|
10.63***
(2.43)
|
-21.83***
(3.47)
|
-11.43***
(3.34)
|
12.71
(15.24)
|
2.87
(14.48)
|
-1.96
(11.52)
|
-0.54
(10.90)
|
Yeas in Germany
|
|
0.04***
(0.001)
|
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
|
0.05***
(0.004)
|
|
0.02***
(0.002)
|
Age
|
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
|
-0.01***
(0.001)
|
|
-0.01***
(0.001)
|
Gender: Female
|
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
|
-0.20***
(0.02)
|
|
-0.06**
(0.03)
|
Observations
|
17,077
|
17,019
|
18,345
|
18,283
|
2,252
|
2,240
|
1,117
|
1,097
|
R-squared
|
0.0682
|
0.2053
|
0.0641
|
0.1932
|
0.1693
|
0.2843
|
0.2851
|
0.3740
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows results of difference-in-differences method (columns 1 to 6). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether the individual can speak German. Measured using SOEP questions, how well can you speak German? Dummy variables are created of individuals who can speak very good and good. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level
Table 6
The effect of ethnic clusters on refugee employment and language adaption across districts with largest refugee numbers
|
Arab
|
Asia
|
|
Employment
(1)
|
Oral language
(2)
|
Employment
(3)
|
Oral language
(4)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
14.74***
(1.41)
|
17.52***
(1.49)
|
11.56***
(1.22)
|
16.73***
(1.26)
|
Post influx
|
-0.14***
(0.04)
|
-0.08***
(0.04)
|
-0.16***
(0.03)
|
-0.15***
(0.03)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
-6.42***
(2.36)
|
-6.55***
(1.95)
|
-2.33***
(1.01)
|
-3.51***
(1.03)
|
Yeas in Germany
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.04***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
Age
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
Gender: Female
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
Observations
|
17,631
|
17,019
|
19,214
|
18,283
|
R-squared
|
0.1991
|
0.2049
|
0.2024
|
0.1932
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows the results of difference-in-differences method for varying ethnic intensity for Arabs (columns 1 to 3), Asians (columns 4 to 6), and European (columns 7 to 9). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
From the table above, the results are found to be consistent with the benchmark analysis. However, for Arab refugees, areas with the highest percentile of ethnic refugee share influence employment and language adaption, measures for economic and social integration, in the opposite direction; negatively. This alarms how the size of the refugee share allocated in districts plays an important role in the integration process.
The size of refugee ethnic share influences the significance and direction of the relationship between ethnic clusters and integration for Arab refugees fleeing war (see columns 1 and 2 of table 5). For instance, when comparing districts with refugee share above the median, the relationship is positive; however, when comparing districts with only the highest refugee share, it has a statistically significant negative effect. This finding justifies the dispersal policies adopted by governments across European nations to tackle ethnic segregation to promote the successful integration of refugees. Nevertheless, the difference in findings across different ethnic backgrounds raises the question about the effectiveness of one for-all policy, for example, the dispersal policy of the Integration Act 2016, restricting the movement of refugees from all ethnic backgrounds in Germany to avoid ethnic segregation. Rather, tailored policies for groups of different demographic, backgrounds, and ethnicity catered to specific characteristics might be more fruitful, as groups might guide individuals’ behavior and self-knowledge (Tajfel and Turner, 1979).
5.1.4 Robustness checks
5.1.4.1 Propensity score matching difference in differences (DID)
Though the allocation of refugees across districts is considered random, as refugees cannot self-select where they will live, the allocation decision might be influenced by various integration factors, such as the availability of housing and support services. Therefore, I used propensity score matching technique to control for the effect of house prices on the assignment of refugees across districts. Propensity scores allow us to estimate the prospect of being assigned to a treatment district based on house price, creating similar treatment and control pairs with similar propensity scores. Propensity score matching validates whether comparison across groups is not biased, functioning as a robustness check of our benchmark results under different assumptions and conditions, justifying whether they are robust under different model specifications.
The common support estimation is used, which mentions the range of values of covariates in overlapping treatment and control groups to ensure a valid comparison between the groups. The region of common support (see row 1) represents the range of values shared among treatment and control groups, providing a more accurate estimate of the treatment effect of the refugee influx. For instance, by limiting the analysis to regions of common support, the estimation is expected to control for potential differences between the groups, providing a more accurate treatment effect estimation. The propensity score mean indicates a higher probability of individuals getting treatment. Lastly, as the covariates show balanced properties, it indicates this analysis is successfull in achieving similar distribution of covariates between treatment and control groups after matching, ensuring the differences in our output variable are due to the refugee influx rather than pre-existing differences.
Table 7
Characteristics of propensity score matching
|
Arab
|
Asian
|
African
|
European
|
Region of common support
|
[0.70, 0.98]
|
[0.31, 0.99]
|
[0.34,0.99]
|
[0.28,0.99]
|
Propensity score means
|
0.77
|
0.88
|
0.63
|
0.59
|
Standard deviation
|
0.03
|
0.06
|
0.14
|
0.11
|
Balance on covariate
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Observations
|
18,053
|
19,680
|
2,361
|
1,437
|
Note: This table shows the charactertistics of propensity score matching, Arabs (columns 1 to 3) and Asians (columns 4 to 6). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not and whether the individual can speak German.
Lastly, I have used Nearest Neighbor Matching method while bootstrapping standard errors to balance the potential imbalance between the treatment and control groups (see table 8).
Table 8
The average treatment effects using the nearest neighbor matching method
|
n Treatment
|
N Control
|
ATT
|
Std error
|
t value
|
Arab
|
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
13888
|
3476
|
0.027
|
0.006
|
4.220
|
Language adaption
|
13888
|
3476
|
0.079
|
0.006
|
12.478
|
Asia
|
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
17287
|
2147
|
0.067
|
0.011
|
5.841
|
Language adaption
|
17287
|
2147
|
0.105
|
0.021
|
5.029
|
Africa
|
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
1500
|
542
|
0.159
|
0.028
|
5.592
|
Language adaption
|
1500
|
542
|
0.105
|
0.047
|
2.242
|
Europeans
|
|
|
|
|
|
Employment
|
846
|
362
|
0.482
|
0.029
|
16.623
|
Language adaption
|
846
|
362
|
0.541
|
0.028
|
19.328
|
Note. This table shows results of average treatment effects using nearest neighbor matching. The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not, and whether the individual can speak German language.
The table above shows the average treatment effects on treated (ATT) estimation using the nearest neighbor matching method (see column 3). The significant t-values indicate that the difference in output variables, employment status, and German language adaption is different between the treatment and control group after matching for covariates. This proves that the refugee influx had a significant effect on the treatment group, further proving the robustness of our model.
5.1.4.2 Falsification test
To test the robustness of the model, two hypothetical scenarios are established, (i) the treatment group is manipulated to contain districts with low refugee share (ii) where the post-influx period is considered as the prior years rather than post-years. I conducted the analysis across Arab and Asian refugee groups. From the results in table 9, it can be observed that the coefficients remain insignificant, and therefore believe that the data supports the assumptions and operations in the model used. The falsification test further proves that the effect on the integration measures is solely due to the refugee influx rather than any other simultaneously occurring factors or events.
Table 9
|
False treatment area – low refugee share and higher
|
False influx year – 2013 and 2014
|
|
Arab
|
Asia
|
Arab
|
Asia
|
|
Employment status
(1)
|
Oral language
(2)
|
Employment status
(3)
|
Oral language
(4)
|
Employment
Status
(5)
|
Oral language
(6)
|
Employment
status
(7)
|
Oral language
(8)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
12.71**
(5.0)
|
23.44***
(4.94)
|
6.71
(5.50)
|
6.71**
(5.50)
|
12.98***
(1.38)
|
15.88***
(1.41)
|
10.30***
(1.18)
|
14.97***
(1.21)
|
Post influx
|
-0.13***
(0.04)
|
-0.04
(0.04)
|
-0.16***
(0.04)
|
-0.16***
(0.04)
|
0.13***
(0.04)
|
0.06***
(0.04)
|
0.14***
(0.03)
|
0.13***
(0.03)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
0.26
(4.62)
|
-7.20
(4.45)
|
3.49
(5.19)
|
3.49
(5.19)
|
-7.54
(15.69)
|
-7.52
(15.83)
|
3.34
(7.03)
|
0.92
(5.78)
|
Yeas in Germany
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.04***
(0.002)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.04***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
Age
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
0.0002)
|
-0.005***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
Gender: Female
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.23***
(0.01)
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
Observations
|
17,631
|
17,019
|
19,214
|
19,214
|
17,631
|
17,019
|
19,214
|
18,283
|
R-squared
|
0.1985
|
0.2045
|
0.2022
|
0.2022
|
0.1985
|
17,019
|
0.2022
|
0.1927
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows results of the difference-in-differences method for falsification tests for Arabs (columns 1,2,5 and 6) and Asians (columns 3,4,7 and 8). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not and whether the individual can speak German language. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
5.1.4.3 Sensitivity test
In this study, a difference in difference (DID) analysis has been used to investigate the influence of ethnic clusters on refugee integration. Nevertheless, due to the sample being limited to only refugees of the SOEP data, the results might be subjected to selection bias. For instance, a significant portion of refugees might be considered as migrants or migrant in the sample might have been naturalized from refugees in the past. Referring to SOEP data, migrants in late 1900s are considered as migrants, however, thousands fled the Yugoslavian wars to Germany during that period, therefore considered to be refugees. Therefore, along with the refugee sample, we include the entire migrant sample.
Table 10
Sensitivity test using larger sample
|
Arabs
|
Asian
|
|
Employment status
(1)
|
Oral language
(2)
|
Employment status
(3)
|
Oral language
(4)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
7.47***
(2.82)
|
3.83
(3.03)
|
19.76***
(2.80)
|
23.63***
(2.51)
|
Post influx
|
-0.13***
(0.04)
|
-0.09***
(0.03)
|
-0.12***
(0.02)
|
-0.15***
(0.01)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
4.66*
(2.21)
|
10.40***
(2.37)
|
-15.69***
(2.30)
|
-12.89***
(1.97)
|
Yeas in Germany
|
0.03***
(0.001)
|
0.04***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.0004)
|
0.02***
(0.0004)
|
Age
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0003)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
-0.01***
(0.0002)
|
Gender: Female
|
-0.24***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
-0.21***
(0.005)
|
-0.14***
(0.01)
|
Observations
|
17,853
|
17,157
|
24,744
|
21,723
|
R-squared
|
0.2001
|
0.2044
|
0.2275
|
0.1906
|
District fixed effects
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Note. This table shows results of the difference-in-differences method for placebo test using random allocation for Arabs (columns 1 and 3), Asians (columns 1 and 3) and Europeans (columns 3 and 6). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not and whether the individual can speak German language. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
The results show the interaction coefficients remain significant and consistent with the benchmark analysis for Arabs, Asians and European samples. Therefore, indicating further robustness of the model used in this study.
5.1.4.4 Placebo test
To further test the robustness of the model, participants from the sample are randomly allocated to either the treatment or control group. The model is run with the new placebo treatment and control groups. As placebos are inactive treatment and control, insignificant results will further indicate the robustness of our model specifications.
Table 11
Placebo test using random allocation
|
Arabs
|
Asian
|
|
Employment status
(1)
|
Oral language
(2)
|
Employment status
(3)
|
Oral language
(4)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers
|
-0.88
(14.37)
|
-7.03
(14.04)
|
-2.43***
(9.60)
|
1.54
(8.46)
|
Post influx
|
-0.23***
(0.06)
|
-0.13***
(0.06)
|
-0.22**
(0.05)
|
-0.22***
(0.05)
|
Ethnic refugee numbers * post influx
|
12.03
(14.00)
|
7.09
(13.79)
|
11.01
(9.39)
|
11.59
(8.20)
|
Age
|
0.03***
(0.00)
|
0.04***
(0.002)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
0.02***
(0.001)
|
Gender: Female
|
-0.01***
(0.0004)
|
-0.01***
(0.0004)
|
-0.01 ***
(0.0004)
|
-0.01***
(0.0004)
|
House price
|
-0.23***
(0.01)
|
-0.17***
(0.01)
|
-0.25***
(0.01)
|
-0.16***
(0.01)
|
Agricultural employment
|
|
|
|
|
Observations
|
9,048
|
8,501
|
10,059
|
9,198
|
R-squared
|
0.2294
|
0.2191
|
0.2388
|
0.2049
|
Note. This table shows results of the difference-in-differences method for placebo test using random allocation for Arabs (columns 1 and 3) and Asians (columns 1 and 3). The observations are at the individual-level unit. The dependent variable measures whether individuals is employed or not and whether the individual can speak German language. Measured using SOEP questions, are you currently employed? Which one of the following applies best to your status? Dummy variables are created of individuals who voted are employed. In parentheses are the robust standard errors. ∗Significant at 10% level; ∗∗significant at 5% level; ∗∗∗significant at 1% level.
The insignificant coefficient for the DID interaction terms supports the robustness of the model. The refugee influx has no significant effect on placebo treatment and control groups, which indicates that the assumptions and model specification is effective.