The studied group
|
Author/year and country
|
Type of document
|
Participants
|
Climate conditions
|
Cognitive performance test
|
Physiological responses
|
Impact of Heat Stress
|
Outcome
|
Quality rating
|
|
Workers
|
Mazloumi et al., 2014 (Iran) (17)
|
Cross-sectional
|
70 M/ Malibel Saipa Company, 35 Case, 35 control
|
Control (WBGT= 16.75-35.4℃)
Case (WBGT= 30.8-32.6℃)
|
SCWT (reaction time, accuracy, and attention)
|
NA
|
Test duration**
Reaction time **
Number of errors ***
|
Heat stress can lead to dysfunction of perceptual abilities, such as selective attention and reaction time.
|
34
|
|
Mazloumi et al., 2014 (Iran) (18)
|
Cross-sectional
|
70 M/ Automobile factory, 35 Cases, 35 control
|
Control (WBGT= 16.75)
Case (WBGT= 30.81-35.41℃)
|
SCWT (reaction time, accuracy, and attention)
|
Stress hormones (Cortisol***; Adrenaline**; Noradrenaline***)
|
Test duration**
Reaction time **
Number of errors ***
|
Heat stress can increase impairment in cognitive performance and stress hormones in blood
|
24
|
|
Golbabaei et al., 2014 (Iran) (19)
|
Cohort
|
70 M/ Hot industry, 35 Case , 35 control
|
Control (WBGT= 16.7)
Case (WBGT= 33℃)
|
SCWT (reaction time, accuracy, and attention)
|
NA
|
Test duration***
Reaction time ***
Increasing frequency of errors ***
Stroop test 3
|
Heat stress can lead to increase the reaction time and decreasing selective attention.
|
24
|
|
Fine et al., 1978 (U.S.A.) (20)
|
Cross-sectional
|
30 M, Artillery fire direction center operations
|
35°C and 88% RH, 7h
|
Complex cognitive tasks
|
NA
|
Increasing the number of omission errors rather than commission errors.
|
Exposing to heat stress affects most of tasks
|
32
|
|
Furtado et al., 2007 (U.S.A.) (21)
|
Cross-sectional
|
12 M, Power industry
|
WBGT= 32.2–37.8 °C
|
Subjective responses, productivity, and error rates
|
Heart rate*
Oxygen consumption***
Tympanic temperature**
|
Productivity***
Subjective responses***
Error rate***
|
Heat stress on outdoor workers leads to fatigue and subsequently deterioration in doing tasks
|
32
|
|
Chen et al., 2003 (Taiwan) (22)
|
Cross-sectional
|
55 M, Electric arc melting workers
|
WBGT=25.4∼28.7°C and 30.0∼33.2°C
|
Fatigue, eyes feel strained, and critical flicker fusion
|
Heart rate
Systolic pressure
|
Critical flicker fusion N. S.
|
Exposing to hot workplace environment leads to workers fatigue.
|
30
|
|
Berg et al., 2015 (U.S.A.) (23)
|
Cross-sectional
|
21 M, Laparoscopic operative tasks ( Surgeons)
|
19 and 26°C
|
SURG-TLX
|
NA
|
Intracorporeal knottying tasks N. S.
|
Hot condition leads to distraction on the score of SURG-TLX among operators.
|
30
|
|
Morley et al., 2012 (USA) (24)
|
Experimental
|
29 M
Firefighters
|
Ta= 33–35°C
|
Neurocognitive tests( short-term memory), sustained and divided attention, and reaction time
|
Heart rate***
Rectal temperature***
|
Short-term memory ***
Reaction time***
|
Neurocognitive tests and reaction time have been detected higher than one hour after exercise
|
28
|
|
Hocking et al., 2001 (Australia)
(25)
|
Experimental
|
Military operations
|
Ta= 25–35°C, RH=65%
|
Brain electrical activity and psychometric test batteries
|
Heart rate****
Rectal temperature****
|
Psychometric task performance*
|
Hostile environments can induce a high sensitivity of brain imaging techniques in cognitive performance.
|
30
|
|
Lan et al., 2011 (China) (26)
|
Experimental
|
6 M and 6W
Office work
|
Ta= 22 and 30°C
|
Neurobehavioral tests, Raw TLX, and tasks typical of office work
|
Heart rate**
Skin temperature
|
Performance**
Mental demand**
|
There is a negative effect on cognitive performance when people feel thermally warm.
|
30
|
|
|
|
Volunteers ( working-age population)
|
Wyon et al., 1979 (Sweden) (27)
|
Experimental
|
36 M and 36 W
|
20—29°C, three successive periods of 50 min with 10-min breaks between
|
Sentence comprehension, Recognition memory
|
NA
|
Sentence comprehension *
Multiplication task *
Recognition memory*
|
Moderate heat stress leads to reducing arousal in the absence of conscious effort.
|
25
|
|
Nunneley et al., 1979 (U.S.A.) (28)
|
Experimental
|
9 M
|
Control ( Td b=25)
Case (WBGT= 31 and 35℃)
|
Compensatory tracking task
|
Heart rate N. S.
Rectal temperature N. S.
|
Simplest tracking task *
Two more difficult tasks N. S.
|
Heat stress can lead to no change in tracking-task execution in cockpit condition simulation
|
28
|
|
Azer et al., 1972 (U.S.A.) (29)
|
Experimental
|
21 M
|
32.2,35 and 37.8 ℃ with RH= 50, 75 and 50% respectively
|
Compensatory tracking task and peripheral task
|
Skin temperature N. S.
Rectal temperature*
Heart rate N. S.
Weight loss rate N. S.
|
Reaction time *
|
The performance was not significantly affected by 3 of climate conditions.
|
30
|
|
Fraser et al., 1955 (U.S.A.) (30)
|
Experimental
|
7 M
|
47.2℃, RH= 50%
|
Simple reaction time and complex reaction times
|
NA
|
Simple reaction time*
|
Experiment showed a negative correlation between simple reaction time and hot workplace environment.
|
28
|
|
Hygge et al., 1991 (Sweden) (31)
|
Experimental
|
32 M and 32 W
|
19 and 27°C
|
Mental arithmetic, a recognition task, hidden figures test, proofreading, verbal fluency, and serial reaction task
|
NA
|
Hidden figures test*
|
There is antagonistic interaction between exposure to heat stress and hidden figures test.
Hot workplace environment leads to increase the rate of response without increase the rate of errors.
|
28
|
|
Provins et al., 1970 (Australian) (32)
|
Experimental
|
20 M
|
WBGT= 35-40℃
|
Visual vigilance task and SRT
|
Oral temperature***
|
Central SRT task*
Peripheral vigilance task*
|
Heat stress can affect perceptual-motor performance by a change in body temperature.
|
30
|
|
Sharma et al., 1986 (India) (33)
|
Experimental
|
8 M, Heat-acclimatized subjects
|
Hot Dry ( Td b =45°C , RH=30% ), and Hot Humid ( Td b =39°C, RH= 60% ) at WBGT= 34°C.
|
Concentration test and psychomotor rest
|
Level of dehydration*
|
Under hot- humid conditions ( Level of dehydration***)
Under hot- dry conditions( Level of dehydration***)
Routine mental work N. S.
|
in hot and humid environments more pronounced concentration component was obvious
|
28
|
|
Taber et al., 2011 (Canada) (34)
|
Experimental
|
5 M and 6 W Helicopter escape
procedures
|
23 °C and RH= 40% , 34 °C and 35%
|
Psychomotor skill (HUET) performance
|
Rectal temperature***
Skin temperature**
Heart rate
|
Performance of HUET skill N. S.
|
Heat stress cannot deficits in escape efficiency that expected in the first time of flight.
|
32
|
|
Gaoua et al., 2011 (Qatar)
(35)
|
Experimental
|
11 M and 5 W
|
CON: 20°C and RH= 40%,
Hot: 50°C and RH= 50%
|
Cognitive testing ( test battery), attention tests, and memory tests
|
Rectal temperature***
Ttymp***
Thead***
|
Cognitive testing**
|
Heat stress exposure can impair workers memory but does not affect simple attentional processes.
|
32
|
|
Cheuvront et al., 2004 (USA) (36)
|
Experimental
|
7 M
|
Ta= 40°C, RH= 20%
|
Cognitive testing ( test battery)
|
V̇O2 peak
Blood Analysis**
|
RPE**
|
Cognitive performance does not alter exerciser in the heat stress exposure when subjects are hypohydrated.
|
30
|
|
Tomporowski et al., 2007 (USA)
(37)
|
Experimental
|
11 M
|
Ta= 30 °C; RH= 40 %
|
Executive-processing test and short-term memory test
|
V̇O2 peak
Rectal temperature**
Skin temperature
Heart rate**
|
Cognitive testing**
RPE**
|
Exercise -induced dehydration influence participants' performance in exposure to heat stress,.
|
32
|
|
Witterseh et al., 2004 (Denmark) (38)
|
Experimental
|
16 M
|
Ta= 22/26/30°C
RH= 45%, 35% and 28%
|
Tsai–Partington test, creative thinking, and text typing
|
NA
|
Tsai–Partington test** Creative thinking**
Text typing***
|
Hot weather can increase subjective distress and fatigue.
|
30
|
|
Færevik et al., 2003 (Denmark) (39)
|
Experimental
|
8 M,
Wearing aircrew protective clothing
|
Ta= 0°C ; RH= 80%,
Ta= 23°C; RH= 63%, and
Ta= 40°C; RH= 19%
|
Cognitive variables (incorrect reactions, missed reactions, and reaction time)
|
At 40°C :
Rectal temperature*
Skin temperature*
Heart rate*
Sweat production*
V̇O2 peak*
|
At 40°C :
Cognitive variables**
|
Cognitive ability is virtually vulnerable to heat stress that cause changing in core body temperature.
|
32
|
|
Athletes
|
Sunderland et al., 2005 (UK) (40)
|
Cross-sectional
|
9 W, Unacclimatized hockey player
|
Hot (30°C, RH= 38% ) and moderate (19°C, RH= 51%)
|
Skill performance, decision-making, fifteen-meter sprint times
|
In the hot environment,
Rectal temperature **
Perceived exertion *
Perceived thirst **
Blood glucose concentration *
Serum aldosterone concentration **
Sweat rate *
|
In the hot environment,
Performance*
Decision-making**
Fifteen-meter sprint times**
|
Field hockey-players showed diminished skill performance in greater environment temperatures in the hot environment conditions.
|
30
|
|
Bandelow et al., 2010 (Turkey) (41)
|
Cross-sectional
|
20 M, Football player
|
33-34°C, RH= 62-65%
|
Cognitive functions, visual
sensitivity
|
Core temperature ***
Plasma osmolality
Glucose levels**
|
Visual sensitivity*
Cognitive functions***
|
Increasing in core body temperature leads to decreasing glucose in blood and leads to diminished faster and also leads to less accurate performance
|
30
|
|