Building upon this initial exploration, the study’s primary objective was to investigate sex differences in cognitive performance and explore the potential association between current levels of sex-specific hormones (oestrogen and testosterone) and these cognitive outcomes. Therefore, we utilised a factor analysis to identify cognitive domains based on the applied cognitive battery developed within the SENDA project [46]. The analysis yielded two distinct domains, namely EF and memory. Consistent with prior research, we found notable sex differences in the identified cognitive domains. Specifically, females exhibited superior performance in EF and memory compared to males. Despite these sex differences, no mediating effects of hormones on cognitive domains were observed, although it is worth noting that oestrogen alone emerged as a significant predictor of EF performance. Additionally, an investigation of the interaction between oestrogen and sex was conducted, considering the direct effects of both variables. The analysis demonstrated a significant relationship between the oestrogen-by-sex interaction and EF, suggesting a stronger association between oestrogen and EF in females than males. Interestingly, as a result of this interaction, the direct effects of sex and oestrogen were attenuated. These findings suggest that oestrogen, which plays a crucial role in shaping and developing the female brain, continues to exert a significant impact on cognitive performance even in older age.
Our study yielded significant findings regarding the impact of sex on the two cognitive domains, namely EF and memory. Consistent with existing literature spanning various age groups, our results indicated that females tend to outperform males in both domains. Sex differences in cognitive performance are generally characterised by small effect sizes [9]. However, it is worth noting that tasks related to visual short-term memory and memory tend to exhibit larger effect sizes [9] For instance, we utilised the Digit Span test, a widely used measure of working memory [48], and the Wordlist Learning test, which evaluates long-term memory [49]. The observed sex differences in cognitive performance may be further amplified due to larger effects and given the critical role of memory in both domains (EF and memory), its impact is likely to contribute to the pronounced disparities between males and females. Moreover, our findings align with recent research conducted by Jockwitz et al. [18], who reported that older females (aged 55 to 87 years) displayed superior performance in verbal fluency, verbal episodic memory, processing speed, and interference tasks. Many of these tasks can be categorised as EFs. Consequently, our study reinforces the notion that healthy older female adults tend to exhibit enhanced performance in tasks that require higher-level of EF or memory capabilities.
It has been well established that during the ageing process, cognition generally declines [36, 37]. However, our study contributes to the existing literature by suggesting that the relative differences between sexes remain consistent as individuals age. Notably, previous research by de Frias et al. [2] examined cognitive performance in a sample ranging from 35 to 80 years old at baseline. Their findings revealed enduring sex differences over a ten-year follow-up period, with women exhibiting superior performance in tasks assessing verbal episodic memory and verbal fluency, while men outperformed women in tasks involving visuospatial functions. Moreover, studies conducted by Maitland et al. [4], Pauls et al. [55], and Jockwitz et al. [18] have further supported the stability of sex differences, particularly in the verbal versus spatial domains, amongst older individuals. While the visuospatial domains were not directly assessed in this study, the derivation of the EF domain through factor analysis, incorporating processing speed, inhibition, attentional control, and working memory, provides a different outlook on cognitive performance that has yet been conducted in previous research. The current findings confirm that collectively differences in cognitive abilities persist even during the later decades of life.
While numerous studies have identified sex differences in cognitive performance among older adults, only a limited number have explored potential biological determinants for these disparities. Oestrogen has emerged as a prominent candidate influencing cognitive performance, supported by various studies [3, 11, 25, 29, 31, 35]. Existing literature has demonstrated an association between a longer reproductive window (reflecting increased exposure to oestrogen) and better cognitive health in later life [56, 57]. In cellular and animal models, oestrogen has exhibited neuroprotective properties, promoting brain-derived neurotrophic factor and enhancing synaptic spine density in the hippocampus [58]. These findings suggest that oestrogen exposure may contribute to the preservation of neural health in late life, which may suggest as to why oestrogen was found to predict EF in older adults.
The lack of an indirect effect of sex through hormones in our study may be attributed to the age of the participants. The current cohort consisted of individuals aged 80 and above, all females were menopausal and not undergoing hormonal replacement therapy. Consequently, the concentration levels of oestrogen did not differ significantly between males and females (albeit being significantly different to younger females [59]). Nonetheless, collectively these findings suggest that the effects of oestrogen on cognitive performance persist throughout the lifespan, and the previous exposure to higher levels of oestrogen concentration may play a more substantial role in explaining the observed sex differences in performance [56, 57].
Previous research has indicated that cognitive differences between the sexes may be influenced by prenatal and neonatal organisational effects of the brain [1, 29, 30, 35]. Sex hormones are known to have organisational effects on the structure and function of specific brain areas during foetal development [60, 61]. Subsequently, during post-pubertal stages, sex hormone levels further enhance the prenatally established neural “hard-wiring” through an activation effect [30]. According to the psychoendocrine theory, substantial quantities of a sex hormone during prenatal life organize neural substrates for specific behaviours or functions. These effects become increasingly evident after puberty when high levels of the same hormone circulate in the body. Consequently, this suggests that oestrogen may continue to benefit cognition due to the retained capacity resulting from its previous ergogenic effects on cognitive function.
To further support this concept, an additional model was conducted, incorporating an oestrogen-by-sex interaction. The results of this analysis demonstrated a significant association with EF, while simultaneously diminishing the individual effects of sex and oestrogen (Fig. 3). These findings suggest a stronger relationship between oestrogen and EF in females compared to males. Therefore, the aforementioned mechanisms throughout the lifespan may account for the enduring sex differences observed in males versus females. Additionally, despite the decline in absolute oestrogen concentrations during ageing, the female brain has a greater number of oestrogen receptors. It could be speculated that due to the affinity with the receptors this may provide the neuroprotective effects mentioned previously alongside the retained cognitive benefits associated with oestrogen. However, it is important to note that these assertions are speculative and should be subject to further investigation in future research. In contrast to the findings for EF, memory displayed sex differences but was not related to oestrogen or testosterone. This might indicate that other biological, but also social or environmental factors and their interactions might partially cause the observed differences between males and females in memory and EF [31].
In the present study, we did not find any cognitive performance measures that showed a significant advantage for males over females. Furthermore, we did not observe any associations between testosterone levels and cognitive performance across various cognitive domains. One plausible explanation for these findings is the well-documented male advantage in tasks involving mental rotation and visual spatial abilities, which has been consistently demonstrated in previous research. However, it is important to note that our study did not include specific tests assessing visual spatial functioning within the cognitive testing battery and across the cognitive domains. As a result, our ability to detect male superiority in cognitive tasks was limited. We did find a significant difference in testosterone levels between males and females, as illustrated in Fig. 2A, which has been suggested to potentially influence performance on visual spatial tasks based on existing literature [27]. Therefore, it would be valuable to explore the relationship between these tasks and testosterone in future research studies employing a similar design.
The current study demonstrated both strengths and limitations. Notably, the study employed confirmatory factor analysis to develop comprehensive variables that capture a holistic perspective of cognition, reflecting real-life cognitive processes instead of focusing on isolated aspects.
Across research the age range of participants often varies widely, leading to substantial variations in cognitive and hormonal states within the population [14, 18]. By exclusively including older adults aged ≥ 80 years, the current study achieved a more homogeneous sample in terms of cognitive and hormonal characteristics compared to other studies. When investigating sex differences, it is crucial to consider the various factors that might contribute to their development.
It is well-established that males and females exhibit differences in brain structure and function, which can impact cognitive processing strategies [20, 60]. In addition, genetic and social factors play a role. The current study extended its investigation to explore the potential hormonal effects on cognitive performance while controlling for other known sex-specific factors such as education level and physical fitness. While the study observed significant differences in cognitive performance favouring females, a limitation arises from the absence of specific cognitive tests tailored specifically for males. Consequently, the cognitive profiles of both sexes may not have been fully represented in the study.
Finally, hormonal concentrations were collected at the same time point for all participants, although there might have been discrepancies between the time elapsed since their last cognitive measures and their hormonal assessments. However, this potential limitation was minimized by the study's recruitment strategy, which targeted a significant age group where hormone fluctuations are not expected to be substantial within the observed timeframes.
PERSPECTIVES AND SIGNIFICANCE
The present study provides further evidence for sex differences in cognitive performance that persist even into late life. Importantly, the study emphasises the potential significance of oestrogen in influencing cognitive abilities, even in post-reproductive life phases. The results demonstrate a direct association between oestrogen and EF, highlighting the crucial role of oestrogen in accounting for the observed sex differences. Particularly noteworthy is the more pronounced oestrogenic effect observed in females (interaction effect), despite the absence of a significant difference in the absolute concentration of oestrogen between males and females. This observation indicates the possibility of either early life or lifelong brain shaping influenced by sex hormones. These findings support the notion that oestrogen may have substantial effects on cognitive development, performance, neuroprotection, and the maintenance of cognitive abilities in females across the lifespan. Therefore, it is essential to incorporate sex hormones, including oestrogen, in research on sex differences, irrespective of age and similar hormone concentration levels. Future investigations should focus on examining the effects of hormones in male-specific cognitive domains (such as mental rotation), as this would provide a more comprehensive understanding of impact on cognitive performance in older adults. Additionally, extending the current findings to pathological conditions such as Alzheimer's disease would be valuable in elucidating the potential role of oestrogen in cognitive impairment.