Study description
Among the 93 articles referenced by the GBD ischemic heart disease meta-analysis, six were excluded because they did not meet the selection criteria (duplicate (n=2), non-English language (n=1), could not be located by a librarian (n=1), and did not explicitly report results from analyses evaluating the impact of alcohol on ischemic heart disease (n=2)). Of the 87 remaining eligible articles (Table S1), 78 were published in a journal with a 2017 JCR impact factor (median 6.1 (interquartile range [IQR], 4.2-18.9)) (Table 2).
Table 2. Characteristics of 87 observational studies evaluating the impact of alcohol consumption on ischemic heart disease
|
|
No. (%)
Median (Interquartile Range)
|
Study characteristics
|
Cohort
|
Case-control
|
Total
|
Number of studies
|
70
|
17
|
87
|
Publication year
|
|
|
|
<1990
|
8 (11.4)
|
2 (11.8)
|
10 (11.5)
|
1990-1999
|
26 (37.1)
|
5 (19.4)
|
31 (35.6)
|
2000-2009
|
25 (35.7)
|
8 (47.1)
|
33 (37.9)
|
2010+
|
11 (15.7)
|
2 (11.8)
|
13 (14.9)
|
Location
|
|
|
|
North America
|
33 (47.1)
|
4 (23.5)
|
37 (42.5)
|
Europe
|
24 (34.3)
|
9 (52.9)
|
33 (37.9)
|
Asia
|
9 (12.9)
|
1 (5.9)
|
10 (11.5)
|
Other
|
4 (5.7)
|
3 (17.7)
|
7 (8.1)
|
Population
|
|
|
|
All
|
30 (42.9)
|
11 (6.5)
|
41 (47.1)
|
Males only
|
33 (47.1)
|
3 (17.7)
|
36 (41.4)
|
Females only
|
7 (10.0)
|
3 (17.7)
|
10 (11.5)
|
Sample size
|
|
|
|
|
11957 (4843-49566)
|
1602 (899-2710)
|
7735 (2634-36191)
|
There were 70 (70 of 87, 80.5%) cohort and 17 (17 of 87, 19.5%) case-control studies (Table 2), which included a median of 11957 (IQR, 4843-49566) and 1602 (IQR, 899-2710) participants, respectively. The majority of the studies were conducted in either North America (37, 42.5%) or Europe (33, 37.9%). Nearly half of the studies included both males and females (41, 47.1%). Two articles did not report results from multivariate regression analyses.
Confounders considered
The largest models in the 85 articles conducting multivariate regression analyses included a median of 9 (IQR, 5-12) adjustment, stratification, and/or matching variables. The vast majority of the 760 total variables were adjustment variables (716, 94.2%); 27 (3.6%) were stratification variables and 17 (2.2%) were matching variables in case-control studies. The 760 variables could be divided into 87 higher-level confounder domains (e.g., “history of angina” and “myocardial infarction” as “heart disease/myocardial infarction/angina”) (Figure S1). The five most commonly considered higher-level domains in the 85 articles were smoking (79, 92.9%), age (74, 87.1%), BMI, height, and/or weight (57, 67.1%), education (39, 45.9%), and physical activity (35, 41.2%) (Figure 1). A total of 33 higher-level domains were only included in one article (Figure S1); 44 were studied at least 3 times. No two articles evaluating the impact of alcohol on ischemic heart disease adjusted, matched, or stratified for the exact same higher-level confounder domains (Figure 2, Figure S2).
Figure 1. The most common higher-level confounder domains considered in 85 observational studies on alcohol and ischemic heart disease risk. Refer to Figure S1 for a larger data microarray.
Figure 2. A “data microarray” illustrating the higher-level confounder domains considered in 85 observational studies on alcohol and ischemic heart disease risk. Domains are ordered based on how many times they were included in multivariate models. Colors represent whether domains were adjustment, stratification, or matching variables and how they were measured. Refer to Figure S2 for a larger data microarray.
The 41 articles evaluating both males and females included a total of 391 variables, which could be divided into 65 higher-level confounder domains. The five most commonly considered higher-level domains in the 41 articles were smoking (40, 97.6%), age (35, 85.4%), sex (31, 75.6%), BMI, height, and/or weight (27, 65.9%), and education (23, 56.1%) (Figure 3, Figure S3). When limited to the 10 articles evaluating only female participants, there were 126 total variables, which could be divided into 37 higher-level confounder domains. The five most common domains were smoking (10, 100.0%), age (10, 100.0%), BMI, height, and/or weight (10, 100.0%), diabetes and diabetes treatment (9, 90.0%), and hypertensions and hypertension drugs (8, 80.0%). The 34 articles with only male participants contained 243 variables, which could be divided into 58 higher-level confounder domains. The five most common domains were smoking (30, 83.3%), age (29, 80.6%), BMI, height, and/or weight (20, 55.6%), and cholesterol/cholesterol treatment (14, 38.9%).
Figure 3. A “data microarray” illustrating the higher-level confounder domains considered in 85 observational studies on alcohol exposure and ischemic heart disease, stratified by the type of population considered. Domains are ordered based on how many times they were included in multivariate models. Colors represent whether domains were adjustment, stratification, or matching variables and how they were measured. Refer to Figure S3 for a larger data microarray.
Among the 74 articles with models that included age, one third (24, 32.0%) were categorical variables (Figure 3), none of which had the exact same age levels. While most (69, 83.1%) of the 83 articles adjusting for smoking included smoking as a categorical variable, only 20 (20 of 69, 29.0%) were measured the exact same way (i.e., never smoking, former smoking, and current smoking). Of the 24 (24 of 65, 36.9%) models that included a measure of BMI as a categorical variable, eight (8 of 24, 33.3%) had cut-off levels that were used in at least one other study (n=2 articles with <20.0, 20.0-24.9, 25.0-29.9, 30.0-34.9, 35.0+ kg/m2 and n=6 articles with <25, 25-29.9, 30+ kg/m2).
Confounding statements and bias considerations
Across all 87 articles, 56 (64.4%) included a specific mention of confounding bias in their Abstract and/or Discussion sections (Table 3). While another 18 (20.7%) articles alluded to the concept of confounding, without using any specific terminology, 13 (14.9%) did not mention or allude to confounding in their Abstract and/or Discussion sections. Over half (50, 57.5%) of the articles used the term “bias”. Among the eight mentions of bias that were related to the principle of confounding, three specifically included the words “confounding” or “confound”.
Nearly one-third (26, 29.9%) of the articles included a discussion regarding potential confounders for which there was no adjustment, and authors frequently (16 of 26, 61.5%) stated that these confounders had not been measured (Table 3).
Table 3. Statements of confounding in studies assessing the impact of alcohol on ischemic heart disease
|
|
Ischemic heart disease
|
Question
|
No. (%, 95% Confidence Interval)
|
Total
|
|
87 (100)
|
Term “Confounding” mentioned in Abstract or Discussion
|
|
|
|
Specific
|
56 (64.4, 54.0-74.7)
|
|
Alluded
|
18 (20.7, 12.6-29.9)
|
|
No
|
13 (14.9, 8.0-23.0)
|
Term “Bias” used in Abstract or Discussion
|
|
|
|
Yes
|
50 (57.5, 47.1-67.8)
|
|
No
|
37 (42.5, 32.2-52.9)
|
Specific mention of non-adjusted confounders
|
|
|
|
Yes
|
26 (29.9, 20.7-40.2)
|
|
Not measured
|
16 (69.6, 42.3-80.8)
|
|
Other reasons
|
5 (21.7, 3.8-34.6)
|
|
No reasons
|
5 (21.7, 3.8-34.6)
|
|
No
|
61 (70.1, 59.8-79.3)
|
Any mention that findings may be affected by confounding?
|
|
|
|
Likely
|
1 (1.2, 0.0-3.4)
|
|
Possibly
|
28 (32.2, 23.0-42.5)
|
|
Unlikely
|
15 (17.2, 9.2-25.3)
|
|
No statement
|
43 (49.2, 39.1-59.8)
|
Cautious interpretation needed
|
|
|
|
Yes
|
5 (5.7, 1.1-11.5)
|
|
No statement
|
82 (94.3, 88.5-98.9)
|
Conclusions include any limitations regarding confounding
|
|
|
|
Yes
|
9 (10.3, 4.6-17.2)
|
|
No
|
78 (89.7, 82.8-95.4)
|
Only one article specifically stated that their main findings were likely to be affected by residual confounding. Another 28 (65.1%) reported it was possible and 15 (34.9%) reported it was unlikely that their main findings were to be affected by residual confounding. There were five (5.7%) that explicitly asked for caution when interpreting results (Table 3).
Articles published after 2010 were more likely to include a specific mention of confounding (12 of 13, 92.3%), use the term “bias” (11 of 13, 84.6%), and ask for caution when interpreting results (2 of 13, 15.4%) (Table S2).