Interest in metacognitive development has started in the early 1970’s by the work of Flavell (1979). He simply defined metacognition as any knowledge or cognitive activity that facilitates and regulates any cognitive activity. This early definition focused on the learners’ knowledge processing skills and cognitive engagement. However, metacognition is a multi-faced notion which includes an individual’s knowledge of self and others and the cognitive processes and strategic skills that include planning, presenting and assessing one’s own cognitive activity (Teng, 2022). Myhill and Jones (2015) examined prominent metalinguistic across research studies (e.g., Gombert, 1992; Jakobson, 1963) however, they adopted Myhill’s (2011, p. 250) metalinguistic definition as “ the explicit bringing into consciousness of an attention to language as an artifact, and the conscious monitoring and manipulation of language to create desired meanings grounded in socially shared understandings”. This definition involves both awareness and the usage of language similar to Kuo and Anderson’s (2008) view that metalinguistic includes both aspects. Metalinguistic awareness is implicitly tied with language usage (Ravid & Tolchinsky, 2002) however many studies assess learners’ awareness through their discourse production.
Metacognition knowledge reflects a high level of language learning. Sato (2022) argued that there are two sides of metacognition development as seen in language learners either as a trait or a state. The difference between these two states can be seen in the language learner’s awareness of learning, rate of progress, recall ability and her quality of cognitive engagement.
The present study focuses on learners’ metacognitive knowledge, as seen in the broader definition of metacognition. Also, metacognitive activity is conceptualized as learners’ declarative knowledge and their engagement during consciousness-raising tasks. Schraw (1994) described three dimensions learners adopt to facilitate their cognitive activity and internalize their metacognition knowledge. The first-dimension learners possess their personal knowledge of their strength and weaknesses of ones own cognitive activity. The second dimension includes task knowledge which is seen in learners’ ability to complete a task successfully whereas the third dimension is strategy knowledge which is the learners’ strategies to complete the tasks successfully. Brown (1987) proposed three sides of metacognition knowledge as including declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge. Declarative knowledge are factors which influence learners’ performance, procedural knowledge manifests one’s own knowledge in applying procedural skills whereas conditional knowledge is knowledge effected by the time and condition that promotes cognitive activity.
Metacognitive knowledge reflects what learners’ know, either formally or informally, explicit or implicit (Teng, 2022). There is a need to provoke this knowledge and make learners conscious of their knowledge so they can easily retrieve their metacognitive knowledge and reveal their awareness. Investigating metacognitive knowledge is reflected in learners of all ages however the scope of this research is on university students (Cotterall & Murray, 2009). Few studies explored young learners’ development of metacognitive knowledge (e.g., Annevirta, Laakkonen, Kinnunen, & Vauras, 2007; Annevirta & Vauras, 2001; Flavell, 1979; Marulis, Palincsar, Berhenke, & Whitebread, 2016; Schneider, 2008; Schneider & Sodian, 1991; Teng & Zhang, 2021). The results mainly showed that children build low levels of metacognitive knowledge during a long period of time. They also, varied individually according to task opportunities, age and early knowledge of declarative and procedural metacognition. On the other hand, older secondary children have more command on their metacognitive knowledge while learning and reflect higher metacognitive knowledge (Edossa, Neuenhaus, Artelt, Lingel, & Schneider, 2019) whereas Schneider, Lingel, Artelt, and Neuenhaus (2017) reported that their learners were more aware in lower secondary grades and that their metacognitive knowledge decreased. Watson, Newman, and Morgan (2021) study collected data from 17 lessons of approximately one hour each, with 10–11-year-old students in their final year of primary school. Through audio capture and transcription of the lessons, the researchers coded the conversations for evidence of declarative knowledge (mentioning or discussing grammar) and procedural knowledge (putting grammar into practice through oral composition of text). The study found that there was very limited evidence of students using terminology to explore writing choices.
Several studies explored undergraduates metacognitive knowledge such as Teng (2020) study investigated how EFL learners metacognition, relates to writing performance among 882 Chinese students at eight universities. The study found that the students' scores on six different aspects of metacognition were positively correlated with their writing performance, and that scores on procedural knowledge, planning, monitoring, and evaluating were particularly important for writing success. Additionally, metacognitive regulation was found to be a unique predictor of writing proficiency beyond the influence of metacognitive knowledge. Therefore, the study suggests that it is crucial to improve metacognitive regulation skills to enhance university EFL learners' writing abilities.
Thanh Son (2022) analysis of the Swedish and Vietnamese learners' declarative and procedural knowledge revealed different profiles. The Swedish learners performed better on the procedural tasks, with 21 of them (62%) using third-person singular present -s at least on two occasions with different verbs. For the Vietnamese learners, only sixteen learners (36%) scored high on the procedural task. The recordings of the learners were transcribed using the CHAT standard as used in the Child Language Data Exchange System.
Hassan, Kasan, Alawawda, and Soliman (2022) study explored the metalinguistic reflective beliefs and teachers' beliefs on grammar content knowledge of Saudi English as a Foreign Language (EFL) teachers. A cross-sectional survey was conducted to assess the reflective beliefs and content knowledge of the teachers using three elements: reflective thinking in the context of grammar instruction, teachers' attitudes about conceptual knowledge, and instructors' satisfaction with the textbook they were using. The results showed that Saudi EFL teachers have a positive view of their metalinguistic reflective beliefs and teachers' beliefs on grammar content knowledge, and that there is a positive relationship between the two variables. Implications of this study will strengthen the language teaching skills of teachers to strengthen the grammar curriculum of the Saudi context.
More research is needed to explore the developmental of metacognitive knowledge and to test the developmental assumptions if such issue in particular domains such as second language grammar to ideally lead to conscious language learners.