Subjects
24 domestic dogs (12 male, 12 female) of various breeds and aged between 1 and 10 years old (m = 5.75, sd = 3.10) participated in this study. An additional 17 dogs were excluded from the study as they were unable to meet the pre-test criteria. To take part the dogs needed to be medium to large size breeds due to the scale of the apparatus used. All subjects were living as pet dogs, and were recruited from a database of dog owners who volunteer their dogs to participate in research at the Dog Cognition Centre, Portsmouth, UK. The owners of the dogs were not present in the room during the study.
Apparatus
Testing took place in a large room (3.7m x 7.58m, see Fig. 1) at the Dog Cognition Centre, Portsmouth. The apparatus (see Fig. 2) comprised of one main board (170cm wide, 85cm high) with two compartments on one side. The compartments had a small opening on the back and at ground level which allowed the dogs to access the food placed inside. The compartments had liftable and interchangeable lids. Both lids were built with an identical wooden frame but either have an opening (transparent side) or were covered with a layer of fly screen and a layer of white backing fabric (opaque side). A handle at the bottom of the lid allowed for the experimenter to easily lift each lid, when the procedure required. Inside each compartment there was white flooring and three small lights, to ensure the food was clearly visible and had the same level of illumination. Additionally, on the lid side of the apparatus a stool was placed, 1.2m back from the main board and equidistance between the two compartments. Tape was used on the floor to mark the dogs’ starting position on the opening side of the apparatus.
Procedure
Throughout the procedure there is one experimenter and one dog handler, both female. The experimenter was the same individual for all participants, but the dog handler varied between dogs.
Pre-test
Every participant dog had to pass the criteria of a pre-test phase to take part in the study. To create the competitive context of the study the leave command was used, therefore only dogs who could follow a ‘leave’ command (or an equivalent command used by the owner) could participate. To test this, in an empty room distinct from the testing room, a piece of food was placed on the floor and the command ‘leave’ was given to the dog. If the dog tried to take the treat, the experimenter would cover the treat with their hand and repeat the command until the dog was able to meet the criteria of waiting, without trying to take the treat, for 30 seconds. If the dog was unable to meet this criterion after 10 minutes, they were excluded from the study.
To ensure the dog understood the competitive nature of the study, and the fact that a human could see them steal from the apparatus when no lids are present, there was a second stage to the pre-test. The participant dog was brought into the testing room, where the apparatus was present with no lids. The dog was led to the openings side and held by the dog handler. The experimenter entered and stood on the lid side of the apparatus, they gained the dog’s attention with a piece of food and then placed it in one compartment. From their side the dog could see it through the corresponding opening. The experimenter then gave the command ‘leave’ in a firm tone. If the dog started to approach the treat the experimenter again said leave and if they continued to approach, they stood up to assert authority, again stating the command. The dog needed to be able to wait 30s without taking the treat at this stage to be able to continue. This was then repeated in the other compartment.
If the dog was able to meet the pre-trial criteria, they were invited to take part in the familiarisation and test phase.
Familiarisation
The familiarisation phase provided the dog with experience of the properties of the lids i.e. transparent or opaque, as well as familiarisation of where they could access the food rewards after they were placed in the compartments. The dog handler led the dog into the room to the lid side of the apparatus, and clipped the dog’s lead onto a wall hook to position them at a distance to watch the handler. The handler showed the dog a treat, saying their name to gain their attention. Then they lifted a lid and placed the food in a compartment. The second lid was lifted to clearly show no treat being present in that compartment. The dog handler then led the dog around to the other side of the apparatus, to a predetermined point centred at equidistance from the openings. At this point the handler gave slack on the lead so the dog could then approach the apparatus and take the treat. If they approached the side without a treat, they were not allowed to then approach the side with the treat, but were guided back to the other side of the apparatus and the next trial started (alternating which side the treat was placed under). This provided the dogs with experience of making a choice between the two openings.
The familiarisation phase consisted of at least 10 repetitions and until the dog had reached the criteria of 5 consecutive correct trials (always choosing the side with the treat present), showing no hesitation taking treats from inside the opening. If they were unable to meet this criterion in 20 repetitions, they were not able to proceed to the testing phase.
Testing
There were two test conditions: Social and non-social.
Social condition
The dog handler followed similar methods to the familiarisation stage. They entered, attached the dog’s lead to the wall hook, and gained their attention with a treat. But unlike the familiarisation stage a treat was placed under both lids. The handler then walked the dog around to the predetermined marked position and at this point an experimenter entered the room and walked to the lid side of the apparatus. They then gained the dogs attention by saying their name, and gave a ‘leave’ command in a firm voice. The experimenter then sat on the stool (once seated their face remained in view of the dog), and focused their gaze at a spot on the main board of the apparatus between the two compartments. The handler then allowed slack on the lead and the dog was able to approach and steal one treat. The handler guided them away before they could try and take a second treat. The condition ended after 120 seconds irrespective of whether the dog did or did not make a choice (take a food item).
Non-social condition
The same procedure as the social condition was followed, however, once the leave command had been given rather than sitting on the stool, the experimenter walked out of the room, closing the door behind them.
The study had a within subject design where each participant dog took part in both of the conditions; social and non-social. Each participant took part in a total of 8 trials, 4 of each condition. To ensure the dog still followed the leave command, despite no repercussions from stealing during trials, the pre-test method in a separate room was repeated after every 2 trials. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced between dogs. The placement of the two lids (i.e. opaque on the left and transparent on the right-hand side) was alternated between participants.
Data analysis
The trials were video recorded and later coded for which compartment the dog stole food from (left or right hand side of the apparatus to check for a side bias), the lid of the compartment they stole from (opaque or transparent), and the latency to steal a piece of food. A secondary coder, unaware of the hypothesis coded 20% of all videos, and had strong intercoder reliability (Spearman = 0.88, n = 39 for Latency, and Kohens kappa = > 0.99, n = 77 for side and lid choice).
Ethical Approval
All the procedures were in accordance with the standards of the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. The study received ethical approval from Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body (AWERB), University of Portsmouth (Reference number 1221E).