Do Cognitive Performance and Physical Function Differ between Individuals with Motoric Cognitive Risk Syndrome and those with Mild Cognitive Impairment?

DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-42676/v1

Abstract

Background

Motoric cognitive risk syndrome (MCR) is defined by slow gait speed combined with subjective cognitive complaint. MCR is a predementia syndrome, similar to mild cognitive impairment (MCI). However, there is currently no study comparing the differences in cognitive performance and physical function between these two types of cognitive impairment. Thus, the aim of this study is to compare cognitive performance and physical function in individuals with MCR versus MCI.

Methods

A total of 77 participants, free of dementia, were recruited from the neurological outpatient clinic of a medical center in Taiwan. Participants were separated into two groups, MCR (n = 33) and MCI (n = 44) groups, based on definition criteria from previous studies. Cognitive performance, including executive function, attention, working memory, episode memory, visuospatial function, and language, were measured. Physical functions such as activities in daily living, the Tinetti Assessment Scale, and the Timed Up and Go test were also measured.

Results

Executive function, attention, working memory, episodic memory and language were all significantly lower in the MCR group than the MCI group. Abilities related to physical function, including those measured by the Tinetti Assessment Scale and the Timed Up and Go test, were significantly lower in the MCR group than the MCI group.

Conclusions

We noted that cognitive performance and physical function were lower in MCR individuals than MCI individuals. The significant differences between those two groups may provide insight that MCR might lead to more severe overall functional deterioration in older adults than MCI patients.

Background

The prevalence of dementia and its associated medical and long-term care costs are growing rapidly internationally as the global population ages [1]. Several studies have shown that cognitive decline, especially in the memory domain, is a common condition of aging and is also considered to be related to the occurrence of dementia [2]. Functional impairment in the cognitive and physical domains occurs with aging and makes elderly individuals vulnerable to adverse events such as disability, falls, or even death [35]. Therefore, it is important to identify and validate biomarkers for the early diagnosis and identification of populations that are at risk of dementia [6].

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a condition that has been studied for 2 decades and corresponds to a state of cognitive function between that of normal aging and dementia [7]. The clinical diagnosis of MCI requires a precise evaluation process, including face-to-face consultations, a series of neuropsychological tests and functional performance tests. This entire assessment takes a substantial amount of time, requires trained medical professionals and is not easy to implement in the community. In the clinical MCI population, the annual conversion rate to dementia has been shown to range from 10–15% [810]. However, the conversion rate in community-dwelling populations is often substantially lower, ranging from 3.8–6.3% per year [1113]. In addition, community-dwelling elderly individuals with MCI are an unstable group, as almost all of them exhibit a change in the functional category each year [13]. Some MCI individuals’ conditions remain stable or even return to normal [14]. This outcome might be incorrectly recorded because of limited resources and the time-consuming nature of the cognitive assessment [15]. Therefore, it may be necessary to expand or modify clinical risk assessments of dementia in community populations.

Several studies have shown that the simultaneous presence of cognitive impairment and gait disturbances is common in elderly individuals [1618]. Additionally, these functional limitations may suggest early signs of dementia [19, 20]. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome (MCR) is new and was proposed by Verghese, et al. [21]; it is characterized by a slow gait speed [1 standard deviation (SD) below the mean age- and sex-specific gait speed] and subjective cognitive complaints. MCR does not require a lengthy comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation, making the assessment highly advantageous in detecting older adults in the community who are at high risk of dementia [21, 22]. In addition, recent studies have shown that MCR can be used to predict the occurrence of disability [23], falls [24] and death [3] in older populations. However, MCR was only recently proposed, and the application and characteristics of MCR are not yet well established.

As mentioned above, MCI and MCR are both predominant cognitive impairment syndromes. However, there are currently no studies comparing cognitive performance and physical function between individuals with these two types of cognitive impairment. Thus, the aim of this study was to compare cognitive performance and physical function in individuals with MCR versus those with MCI. MCR has been shown to have improved predictive validity for dementia [18] and falls [24] compared to its individual cognitive and motor components. Therefore, we assumed that older adults suffering from MCR have poorer cognitive and physical performance than do those with MCI. Identifying the differences in functional capacity between individuals with MCR and MCI may help us understand the difference between these two syndromes and predict dementia in elderly individuals.

Methods

Participants and study design

This study was a cross-sectional study conducted in Taiwan between April 2018 and August 2018. Participants were recruited from a medical center in Taipei. All participants met the following criteria: (a) aged 60 years or older, (b) could walk 10 meters independently, (c) were community dwelling, (d) had subjective cognitive complaints and (e) were not diagnosed with dementia. The presence of subjective cognitive complaints was determined by a ‘yes’ response to the memory item on the Geriatric Depression Scale [25, 26]. Dementia was diagnosed after all clinical and neuropsychological information presented at diagnostic case conferences were reviewed. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) unstable medical conditions, for example, the presence of major visual or hearing loss, (b) a recent or planned surgery leading to limitations in walking and interfering with participation in this study or (c) the consumption of any medications causing cognitive complaints during the past 3 months. A total of 86 participants provided informed consent, and the study procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Mackay Memorial Hospital (number: 18MMHIS005e). We confirm that all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations. The subject inclusion process for this study is depicted in Fig. 1 (n = 77). The participants’ age, sex, history of metabolic disease, and subjective cognitive complaints were obtained from patient interviews and medical charts.

Study assessment

The selected participants underwent comprehensive neuropsychological testing and physical activity testing with the Barthel Index, Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) scale, Tinetti Assessment Tool and Timed Up and Go (TUG) test.

A face-to-face neuropsychological assessment was performed using the Chinese versions of parts A and B of the Trail Making Test (TMT) [27], the category fluency test [28], digit recall forwards and backwards [29], the Chinese version of the California Verbal Learning Test [30], the short form of the Judgment of Line Orientation test [31], the Boston Naming Test [32], and the Chinese version of the Geriatric Depression Scale-15 (GDS-15) [33]. The TMT is a neuropsychological test of visual attention and executive function [34]. There were two parts for this test: in the first part, participants were instructed to draw a line connecting numbers in sequential order (1, 2, 3, etc. ); in the second part, the participants had to alternate between numbers and letters (1, A, 2, B, etc.). The time taken to complete the test was used as the performance indicator. The category fluency test was designed to measure executive functions and language [35]. Participants were asked to provide as many words as possible from a category (e.g., animals) in 60 seconds. Digit recall forwards and backwards are the most widely used short-term verbal memory and executive function tests [29]. Participants were asked to either recall a sequence of numerical digits in normal or reverse order correctly, with increasingly longer sequences being tested in each trial. The longest number of sequential digits that can accurately be remembered was recorded as the participant’s performance. The Chinese version of the California Verbal Learning Test is designed to measure episodic memory for the Chinese-speaking population aged ≥ 50 years old [30]. This test was composed of 9 two-character nouns presented over 4 learning trials. After each trial, the participant was asked to recall as many words as possible in any order. The number of correct items was recorded in each trial. The short form of the Judgment of Line Orientation test is a 10-item standardized test of visuospatial performance [36]. Participants were asked to match two angled lines to a set of 11 lines that were arranged in a semicircle and separated by 18 degrees. The number of correct lines was recorded as the performance indicator. The Boston Naming Test, consisting of 30 black and white line drawings of objects, is one of the most commonly used measures of confrontation naming in individuals with language impairments caused by diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease and dementia [32]. The participants were asked to state the name of the object in each picture, and the examiner recorded the number of correct items. The Chinese version of the Geriatric Depression Scale-15 is a valid assessment tool used to evaluate the prevalence of depressive symptoms among the Chinese rural elderly people [33]. The scale consists of 15 yes-no questions, and each question is scored with either 0 or 1 point. Scores of 0–4 points are considered normal; those of 5–8 points indicate mild depression; those of 9–11 points indicate moderate depression; and those of 12–15 points indicate severe depression.

Functional status was assessed using the Barthel Index for self-care activities such as washing, dressing, grooming, using the restroom, and eating and the IADL scale for activities such as cooking, shopping, doing laundry, and maintaining household finances. Physical function was also assessed by the Tinetti Assessment Scale, which is a test that is easy to perform and measures balance ability and gait [37]. The gait speed and TUG tests were conducted with G-WALK® (BTS Bioengineering Corp., MA, United States), which is a wearable system used for the functional analysis of movement. This system comprises a portable inertial sensor that is attached to the S1-S2 vertebrae and records specific movements. When a subject walks, the sensor collects and transmits data to a laptop for analysis. The sampling rate of the system is 100 Hz. The validity and reliability of this system to assess movement performance has been well established [38]. Gait speed was evaluated during walking at a habitual speed. The participants were asked to walk 10 meters at a comfortable speed three times, and the average speed of the three trials was used for data analysis. For the TUG test, the participants were instructed to stand up from a chair, walk 3 meters, turn around, walk back to the chair, and sit down a total of three times. The time needed to complete the task was recorded, and the average time of the three trials was used for data analysis.

After completing all assessments, the participants were separated into two groups, the MCR (n = 33) and MCI (n = 44) groups, based on the criteria reported in a previous study [12, 21]. MCR was diagnosed if participants met all of the following four criteria [21]: (a) subjective cognitive complaints, (b) a slow gait speed, (c) the absence of dementia, and (d) a consistent level of independence in activities of daily living. Subjective cognitive complaints was defined as reported in the inclusion criteria [25, 26]. Based on previous studies, a slow gait speed was defined as a walking speed of one SD or more below the mean age- and sex-specific gait speed [21, 39]. MCI was diagnosed using the following criteria[40]: (a) subjective cognitive complaints, (b) objective cognitive impairment in one or more cognitive domains, (c) preserved activities of daily living, and (d) the absence of a diagnosis of dementia.

Statistics

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 22.0 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, United States). The participants’ characteristics were summarized using the means and SDs or numbers, as appropriate. Between-group comparisons were performed using independent t-tests (continuous variables) or chi-square tests (categorical variables). In this study, the correlations were first established, and the factors correlating significantly with gait speed were further analyzed using a linear regression model. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used to examine correlations between cognitive performance and gait speed. We considered correlation coefficients larger than 0.3 as meaningful correlations [41, 42]. In addition, because the Pearson’s correlations were examined 11 times, the significance level was corrected with the Bonferroni method (p = 0.005) to reduce the possibility of statistical errors. A linear regression model (stepwise strategy) was used to determine the contributions of different cognitive domains to gait speed. A p value of less than 0.05 indicated statistical significance.

Results

Baseline demographic data

Seventy-seven subjects (males: 43; females: 34) participated in the study. Table 1 presents the characteristics of the participants in the MCR (n = 33) and MCI (n = 44) groups. The mean ages of the participants in the MCR and MCI groups were 69.3 ± 9.8 and 70.0 ± 8.8 years, respectively. There were no differences in the basic characteristics (age, sex, body mass index, and educational level) or the prevalence of a medical condition (hypertension, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, and heart disease) between the subjects with MCR and those with MCI. The participants with MCR had a lower gait speed than did the MCI participants (p < 0.001).

Table 1

Comparison of participants’ characteristics between the two groups.

 

Motoric cognitive risk syndrome (n = 33)

Mild cognitive impairment (n = 44)

P value

Age (years)

69.3 ± 9.8

70.0 ± 8.8

0.733

Sex (male/female)

17/16

26/18

0.643

BMI

24.4 ± 3.4

23.9 ± 3.3

0.480

Educational level (years)

7.6 ± 3.7

7.9 ± 4.3

0.764

Gait speed (m/s)

0.7 ± 0.1

1.0 ± 0.2

< 0.001

Hypertension (n)

19

30

0.351

Diabetes (n)

12

14

0.808

Hyperlipidemia (n)

19

29

0.485

Heart disease (n)

11

16

0.814

Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index.

Comparisons between MCR and MCI

The cognitive performance and physical function of the participants with MCR and MCI are shown in Table 2. Compared to the subjects with MCI, those with MCR had poorer executive function (TMT-A, p = 0.034; TMT-B, p = 0.043; category fluency test, p = 0.003), a poorer memory ability (digit recall backwards, p = 0.016; California Verbal Learning Test-short form, p = 0.030), poorer language function (Boston Naming Test, p = 0.027), and more depressive symptoms (GDS-15, p = 0.028). In addition, the Barthel Index and IADL results were similar between the two groups, but the subjects with MCR had significantly lower Tinetti gait and balance scores (gait, p = 0.010; balance, p = 0.028) and a longer TUG completion time (p < 0.001) than did the MCI group.

Table 2

Comparison of the participants’ cognitive performance and functional status between the two groups.

 

Motoric cognitive risk syndrome (n = 33)

Mild cognitive impairment (n = 44)

P value

Executive function

     

TMT-A (s)

30.0 ± 16.3

21.9 ± 16.2

0.034

TMT-B (s)

75.2 ± 39.1

59.0 ± 34.6

0.043

Category fluency test

10.8 ± 3.3

13.4 ± 3.9

0.003

Attention and working memory

   

Digit recall forwards

7.2 ± 1.4

7.3 ± 1.6

0.764

Digit recall backwards

3.5 ± 1.1

4.3 ± 1.7

0.016

Episode memory

California Verbal Learning Test-short form

17.6 ± 5.0

20.1 ± 5.2

0.030

Visuospatial performance

   

Judgment of Line Orientation

13.2 ± 4.1

13.8 ± 3.2

0.522

Language

     

Boston Naming Test

20.9 ± 5.3

23.6 ± 4.9

0.027

Depression

 

Geriatric Depression Scale-15

3.9 ± 3.7

2.1 ± 2.6

0.028

Physical function

     

Barthel Index

96.1 ± 10.8

99.9 ± 0.8

0.057

IADL

20.7 ± 5.3

22.3 ± 3.9

0.150

Tinetti gait

10.9 ± 2.0

11.9 ± 0.6

0.010

Tinetti balance

14.2 ± 3.4

15.6 ± 1.1

0.028

Timed Up and Go test (s)

22.7 ± 13.4

13.4 ± 3.1

< 0.001

Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; IADL, Instrumental Activities of Daily Living.

Association of cognitive performance and gait speed

Table 3 shows the correlation results. The scores of the category fluency test, digit recall backwards test and short form of the California Verbal Learning Test were positively correlated with gait speed (r = 0.370–0.449, p < 0.005). The TMA-A scores were negatively correlated with gait speed (r = -0.376, p < 0.005). According to the regression models (Table 4), executive function was the most important factor in determining gait speed (Model 1: F = 14.02, P < 0.001, effect size f2 = 0.21, statistic power = 0.98; Model 2: F = 10.91, P < 0.001, effect size f2 = 0.33, statistic power = 0.99).

Table 3

Correlation coefficient between cognitive performance and gait speed.

 

Gait speed, r

P value

Age (years)

-0.219

0.056

Educational level (years)

0.171

0.138

Executive function

TMT-A (s)

-0.376

0.001*

TMT-B (s)

-0.324

0.005

Category fluency test

0.449

< 0.001*

Attention and working memory

Digit recall forwards

0.172

0.135

Digit recall backwards

0.370

0.001*

Episode memory

California Verbal Learning Test-short form

0.391

< 0.001*

Visuospatial performance

Judgment of Line Orientation

0.085

0.467

Language

Boston Naming Test

0.305

0.007

Depression

GDS-15

-0.292

0.010

Abbreviations: TMT, Trail Making Test; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale. *p < 0.005.

Table 4

Summary of linear regressions.

Variable

β

p

Model 1

   

Category fluency test

0.413

< 0.001

R square

0.171

 

Adjusted R square

0.159

 

P value

< 0.001

 

Model 2

   

Category fluency test

0.342

0.003

Trail Making Test part A (s)

-0.283

0.012

R square

0.246

 

Adjusted R square

0.223

 

P value

< 0.001

 
Variables eliminated from regression model 1: California Verbal Learning Test, Digit recall backwards, Trail Making Test part A.
Variables eliminated from regression model 2: California Verbal Learning Test, Digit recall backwards.

Discussion

In the present study, it was demonstrated that compared to individuals with MCI, subjects with MCR are more likely to exhibit executive function, attention, memory, and language impairments. Our results also showed that physical functions, including gait and balance performance, are significantly lower in people with MCR than in those with MCI. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to compare cognitive performance and physical function in individuals with two types of cognitive impairment, MCR and MCI. We also found that executive function is the primary factor associated with gait speed compared with attention, memory, visuospatial, and language performance.

A major contribution of this study to the literature includes the results of cognitive performance and physical function in individuals with MCR versus those with MCI. Recent reports have shown that the simultaneous presence of cognitive decline and a slow walking speed can predict dementia [19, 20]. MCR is a novel concept and is defined as a heterogeneous clinical manifestation characterized by the simultaneous presence of an objectively slow gait speed and subjective cognitive complaints [21, 43]. Individuals with MCI demonstrate cognitive impairment with minimal impairment in IADL [44]. Both conditions are linked to poor cognitive function and represent a stage of predementia [22]. However, currently, the information on the characteristics of MCR and the differences between MCR and MCI are still limited. In the present study, we found that compared to MCI, MCR is linked not only to poorer physical function but also to poorer cognitive performance in tasks related to executive function, attention, working memory, episode memory, and language. This result indicates that compared with MCI, MCR might lead to more severe overall functional deterioration in older adults. In a three-year longitudinal study, people with MCR were found to be at higher risk of developing dementia than were people with not MCR (hazard ratio: 3.27) [21]. A random effects meta-analysis reported that the relative risk of dementia was 3.3 in those with MCI versus age-matched controls 2–5 years later [45]. Although the conversion rates in these two groups are similar, the research designs and the participants included in these studies differed greatly. Since we found significant differences in cognitive performance and physical function between MCR and MCI patients, it is important that a large cohort study is conducted in the future to compare the rate of conversion to dementia between these two groups.

Based on the definitions of MCR and MCI, both are predementia cognitive disorders, and the factor that differentiates them is gait speed. Walking is a very common activity of daily living and is a consequence of multifactorial and multisystem (sensory, musculoskeletal, nervous, and cardiorespiratory) coordination. Executive function [16, 46] and attention [46, 47] are considered to be the most important cognitive domains affecting gait performance. Our research findings reported that people with MCR who had a slower gait speed also had poorer executive function, poorer attention and memory performance, poorer language function, and more depressive symptoms than did participants with MCI. In addition, we also noted that executive function was the primary factor associated with gait speed compared with other domains of cognition, which is consistent with other research results [48, 49]. A growing body of evidence suggests that frontal subcortical circuits mainly control the speed of walking [50]. Moreover, compromised frontal lobe white matter was found to be associated with executive network functional impairments and slower walking speeds in elderly individuals [51, 52]. These brain areas are highly susceptible to white matter hyperintensities, microvascular damage, and neurodegenerative pathologies, which are common precursors of dementia [52]. In summary, brain regions and networks specifically control both higher-level cognitive function and gait performance and explain the relationship between a slow walking speed and dementia pathologies.

Regarding physical function, our results showed that the mean Tinetti Assessment Scale score in subjects with MCR was significantly lower than that in people with MCI, indicating that individuals with MCR not only walk slowly but also have poor balance. Moreover, compared to the MCI group, the MCR group needed more time to complete the TUG test (mean, MCI group = 13.4 seconds; MCR group = 22.7 seconds, p < 0.001). The TUG test is a reliable measure of functional activity that captures transfers, straight walking, and turning movements [53]. It has been reported that this test can be used to identify elderly individuals who are prone to falls (completion time of ≥ 14 seconds), with a sensitivity of 0.87 and a specificity of 0.87 [54]. This result demonstrates that our MCR group had a high-risk for falling, which is consistent with other research results [24]. As mentioned above, maintaining a normal well-balanced gait is a complex process requiring the efficient cooperation of multiple systems, such as those controlling motor, cognitive, and sensory processes [16, 20, 55], and the inability to have a normal gait can lead to falls. We therefore suggest that MCR is an effective screening tool for the risk of falls in older populations.

According to our results, people with MCR have poorer cognitive function and physical function than do MCI patients, indicating that MCR might lead to more severe overall functional deterioration than MCI in older adults. MCR does not require a time-consuming comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, unlike MCI, which is convenient for the clinical evaluation of the risk of dementia in community-dwelling older populations. In addition, compared with people with MCI, people with MCR might have a higher risk of falling, based on the completion time for the TUG test. There are some study limitations that should be addressed in the present study. The inclusion of a relatively small number of participants made it impossible to perform any subgroup analyses with sufficient power, such as an MCI subtype comparison. Studies with a larger sample size should be conducted in the future to confirm our results and to conduct subgroup analyses. We also used a cross-sectional study design, which prevented us from investigating changes in cognitive and physical function in the two groups over time. To better understand the actual interaction between cognitive and physical functions in aging populations that are at high risk of dementia, a large cohort study is warranted and encouraged. Moreover, it should be noted that since MCR is defined as a condition characterized by a slow gait speed in the presence of subjective cognitive complaints in elderly people not diagnosed with dementia or mobility impairments, the MCR subjects included in this study may or may not have met the MCI diagnosis. In this study, we did not exclude MCR subjects with MCI, which may have affected our results. However, the definition of MCR does not include conditions that can exclude subjects with MCI [21]. Therefore, the characteristics and differences between MCI and MCR need further clarification.

Conclusion

This study suggests that cognitive performance and physical function are lower in MCR individuals than in MCI individuals and that executive function, compared with other cognitive domains, is the primary factor associated with gait speed. The significant differences between these two groups suggest that MCR leads to more severe overall functional impairment than does MCI in older adults. Additional studies on the mechanisms of MCI and MCR and subsequently, longitudinal studies on these predementia syndromes would be worthwhile.

Abbreviations

MCR: Motoric cognitive risk syndrome; MCI: mild cognitive impairment; SD: standard deviation; IADL: Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; TUG: Timed Up and Go; TMT: Trail Making Test; GDS-15: Geriatric Depression Scale-15.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The project was conducted in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of Mackay Memorial Hospital (number: 18MMHIS005e). An overseeing neurologist has ruled that all participants have been deemed capable of ethically and medically consenting for their participation in the research presented in this manuscript. Written informed consent was obtained from participants prior to the start of data collection.

Consent to publish

Not applicable.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from the Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST 108-2314-B-715-003) and Mackay Memorial Hospital (MMH-MM-10811, and NTUT-MMH-108-06). The funders had no role in the design of the study, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data, or writing of the manuscript.

Author’s contributions

F.-Y.C. conducted the experiment, analyzed the results and wrote the manuscript, Y.C. and J.-S.S. conceived the experiment, S.-J.C. and C.-Y.L. recruited the subjects, P.-H.C. conceived the experiment and reviewed the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the participants, as well as the research assistants Yi-Hsuan Ho, Wen-Chun Wu, and Lu-Shan Lee, for participating in the assessment.

References

  1. World Health Organization. Global dementia observatory, http://apps.who.int/gho/data/node.dementia?lang=en.
  2. Jonker C, Geerlings MI, Schmand B. Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15:983–91.
  3. Ayers E, Verghese J. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and risk of mortality in older adults. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12:556–64.
  4. Clegg A, Young J, Iliffe S, Rikkert MO, Rockwood K. Frailty in elderly people. Lancet. 2013;381:752–62.
  5. Montero-Odasso M, Verghese J, Beauchet O, Hausdorff JM. Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:2127–36.
  6. Handels RLH, Wimo A, Dodel R, Kramberger MG, Visser PJ, Molinuevo JL, Verhey FRJ, Winblad B. Cost-utility of using Alzheimer's disease biomarkers in cerebrospinal fluid to predict progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;60:1477–87.
  7. Petersen RC, Stevens JC, Ganguli M, Tangalos EG, Cummings JL, DeKosky ST. Practice parameter: early detection of dementia: mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2001;56:1133–42.
  8. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:303–8.
  9. Bozoki A, Giordani B, Heidebrink JL, Berent S, Foster NL. Mild cognitive impairments predict dementia in nondemented elderly patients with memory loss. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:411–6.
  10. Tierney MC. Cognitive tests that best discriminate between presymptomatic AD and those who remain nondemented. Neurology. 2001;57:163–4.
  11. Grober E, Lipton RB, Hall C, Crystal H. Memory impairment on free and cued selective reminding predicts dementia. Neurology. 2000;54:827–32.
  12. Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, McKeel DW, Price JL, Rubin EH, Berg L. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:397–405.
  13. Ritchie K, Artero S, Touchon J. Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology. 2001;56:37–42.
  14. Petersen RC. Clinical practice. Mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:2227–34.
  15. Villars H, Oustric S, Andrieu S, Baeyens JP, Bernabei R, Brodaty H, Brummel-Smith K, Celafu C, Chappell N, Fitten J, Frisoni G, Froelich L, Guerin O, Gold G, Holmerova I, Iliffe S, Lukas A, Melis R, Morley JE, Nies H, Nourhashemi F, Petermans J, Ribera Casado J, Rubenstein L, Salva A, Sieber C, Sinclair A, Schindler R, Stephan E, Wong RY, Vellas B. The primary care physician and Alzheimer's disease: an international position paper. J Nutr Health Aging. 2010;14:110–20.
  16. Cohen JA, Verghese J, Zwerling JL. Cognition and gait in older people. Maturitas. 2016;93:73–7.
  17. Robertson DA, Savva GM, Kenny RA. Frailty and cognitive impairment–a review of the evidence and causal mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12:840–51.
  18. Montero-Odasso M, Speechley M, Muir-Hunter SW, Sarquis-Adamson Y, Sposato LA, Hachinski V, Borrie M, Wells J, Black A, Sejdic E, Bherer L, Chertkow H, Canadian Gait and Cognition Network. Motor and cognitive trajectories before dementia: results from gait and brain study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2018;66:1676–83.
  19. Prince M, Bryce R, Ferri C. (2011) World Alzheimer Report 2011: The Benefits of Early Diagnosis and Intervention, Alzheimer’s Disease International.
  20. Borges SM, Radanovic M, Forlenza OV. Correlation between functional mobility and cognitive performance in older adults with cognitive impairment. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2018;25:23–32.
  21. Verghese J, Wang C, Lipton RB, Holtzer R. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:412–8.
  22. Verghese J, Annweiler C, Ayers E, Barzilai N, Beauchet O, Bennett DA, Bridenbaugh SA, Buchman AS, Callisaya ML, Camicioli R, Capistrant B, Chatterji S, De Cock AM, Ferrucci L, Giladi N, Guralnik JM, Hausdorff JM, Holtzer R, Kim KW, Kowal P, Kressig RW, Lim JY, Lord S, Meguro K, Montero-Odasso M, Muir-Hunter SW, Noone ML, Rochester L, Srikanth V, Wang C. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: multicountry prevalence and dementia risk. Neurology. 2014;83:718–26.
  23. Doi T, Shimada H, Makizako H, Tsutsumimoto K, Verghese J, Suzuki T. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: association with incident dementia and disability. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59:77–84.
  24. Callisaya ML, Ayers E, Barzilai N, Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Lipton RB, Otahal P, Srikanth VK, Verghese J. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and falls risk: a multi-center study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;53:1043–52.
  25. Ayers E, Verghese J. Gait dysfunction in motoric cognitive risk syndrome. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;71:95–103.
  26. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, Lum O, Huang V, Adey M, Leirer VO. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17:37–49.
  27. Wu YH, Liu LK, Chen WT, Lee WJ, Peng LN, Wang PN, Chen LK. (2015) in J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc..
  28. Rosen WG. Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. J Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;2:135–46.
  29. Richardson JT. Measures of short-term memory: a historical review. Cortex. 2007;43:635–50.
  30. Chang CC, Kramer JH, Lin KN, Chang WN, Wang YL, Huang CW, Lin YT, Chen C, Wang PN. Validating the Chinese version of the verbal learning test for screening Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:244–51.
  31. Mitrushina M, Boone KB, Razani J, D'Elia LF. Handbook of Normative Data for Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.
  32. del Toro CM, Bislick LP, Comer M, Velozo C, Romero S, Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Kendall DL. Development of a short form of the Boston naming test for individuals with aphasia. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2011;54:1089–100.
  33. Liu CY, Wang SJ, Teng EL, Fuh JL, Lin CC, Lin KN, Chen HM, Lin CH, Wang PN, Yang YY, Larson EB, Chou P, Liu HC. Depressive disorders among older residents in a Chinese rural community. Psychol Med. 1997;27:943–9.
  34. Arnett JA, Labovitz SS. Effect of physical layout in performance of the trail making test. Psychol Assess. 1995;7:220–1.
  35. Brucki SM, Rocha MS. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37:1771–7.
  36. Calamia M, Markon K, Denburg NL, Tranel D. Developing a short form of Benton's judgment of line orientation test: an item response theory approach. Clin Neuropsychol. 2011;25:670–84.
  37. Tinetti ME. Performance-oriented assessment of mobility problems in elderly patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1986;34:119–26.
  38. BTS Bioengineering. G-WALK®_UserManual. USA: BTS Bioengineering; 2016.
  39. Verghese J, Ayers E, Barzilai N, Bennett DA, Buchman AS, Holtzer R, Katz MJ, Lipton RB, Wang C. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: multicenter incidence study. Neurology. 2014;83:2278–84.
  40. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–94.
  41. Hinkle D, Wiersma W, Jurs S. Applied Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 2003.
  42. Mukaka MM. Statistics corner: a guide to appropriate use of correlation coefficient in medical research. Malawi Med J. 2012;24:69–71.
  43. Ayers E, Verghese J. Diagnosing motoric cognitive risk syndrome to predict progression to dementia. Neurodegener Dis Manag. 2014;4:339–42.
  44. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–94.
  45. Petersen RC, Lopez O, Armstrong MJ, Getchius TSD, Ganguli M, Gloss D, Gronseth GS, Marson D, Pringsheim T, Day GS, Sager M, Stevens J, Rae-Grant A. Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment: Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2018;90:126–35.
  46. Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov Disord. 2008;23:329–42. quiz 472.
  47. Parihar R, Mahoney JR, Verghese J. (2013) in Curr. Transl. Geriatr. Exp. Gerontol. Rep.
  48. Atkinson HH, Rosano C, Simonsick EM, Williamson JD, Davis C, Ambrosius WT, Rapp SR, Cesari M, Newman AB, Harris TB, Rubin SM, Yaffe K, Satterfield S, Kritchevsky SB, Health ABC Study. Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities: the health, aging and body composition study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:844–50.
  49. Watson NL, Rosano C, Boudreau RM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Hardy SE, Atkinson HH, Yaffe K, Satterfield S, Harris TB, Newman AB, Health ABC, Study. Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65:1093–100.
  50. Holtzer R, Verghese J, Xue X, Lipton RB. Cognitive processes related to gait velocity: results from the Einstein Aging Study. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:215–23.
  51. Poole VN, Wooten T, Iloputaife I, Milberg W, Esterman M, Lipsitz LA. Compromised prefrontal structure and function are associated with slower walking in older adults. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;20:620–6.
  52. Montero-Odasso M, Hachinski V. Preludes to brain failure: Executive dysfunction and gait disturbances. Neurol Sci. 2014;35:601–4.
  53. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed "Up & Go": a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1991;39:142–8.
  54. Shumway-Cook A, Brauer S, Woollacott M. Predicting the probability for falls in community-dwelling older adults using the timed up & go test. Phys Ther. 2000;80:896–903.
  55. Rosso AL, Studenski SA, Chen WG, Aizenstein HJ, Alexander NB, Bennett DA, Black SE, Camicioli R, Carlson MC, Ferrucci L, Guralnik JM, Hausdorff JM, Kaye J, Launer LJ, Lipsitz LA, Verghese J, Rosano C. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:1379–86.