The active imagination of movements also called motor imagery (MI) is defined as the mental representation of an action without engaging in its actual execution (1).
In sport psychology, studies frequently reported that MI has a positive effect on motor performance (2, 3). Over the last decades, MI has been successfully introduced to a wide range of disciplines, e.g.in education, medicine, and music (4). In addition, the MI technique has been successfully integrated in rehabilitation because it facilitates positive neurophysiological changes in the central nervous system and therefore, brain plasticity (5, 6). Neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) demonstrated that physical execution and mental rehearsal of a movement show similar brain activation pattern (7, 8). In neurological rehabilitation, MI seems to be an effective complementary therapy for patients with sensorimotor impairments providing additional benefits if added to conventional therapies (9–11). Patients seem to advantage from MI because it can be safely practiced alone, thus expanding the time spent with rehabilitation-related activities (12).
There are two different perspectives described to imagine a movement: a) an internal and b) an external perspective. The internal view describes the first-person view, where the imaginer visualises the movement through his or her own view, comparable to what you would see through a camera mounted on the head. The external perspective describes the third-person view, where the imaginer watches him or herself performing an action from a spectator’s position (13, 14).
So far, preference was given to the internal perspective because it is believed to be more effective than external perspective MI. Using fMRI technology, Lorey, et al. observed that the internal perspective leads to a stronger activation of motor and motor-related areas than the external perspective (15). Several studies have investigated the effectiveness of MI using first person perspective imagery for rehabilitation purposes (16–18). Furthermore, in a review on MI training elements, Schuster, et al. reported, that in MI interventions with a positive results, internal perspective was more frequently used (4). Also, the Kinaesthetic and Visual Imagery Questionnaire-20, which is standardized questionnaire to assess MI ability of patients with sensorimotor impairments, emphasizes the first person perspective (19).
Only few studies investigated the explicit effects of MI perspectives on motor performance (13, 20, 21). However, the available findings on MI perspective do not support the notion that the use of an internal perspective is consistently superior to an external perspective (20, 21). In a sport context, White and Hardy (20) demonstrated that different MI perspectives could enhance different aspects of motor performance. External imagery was found to be more effective for learning and retention of a task and improvement of speed, whereas internal imagery supported accuracy of performance (20). Furthermore, there is evidence that suggests athletes rather take on an external perspective in open sports with complex movements (21, 22). So far, the basis of the recommendation to select the internal or the external MI perspective is inconclusive.
Less is known about how patients with sensorimotor impairments imagine movements. Studies on MI involving patients focused on MI ability, and reported fewer details concerning MI perspectives. Randhawa et al. described that patients with Parkinson’s disease showed some difficulty to imagine axial movements (e.g. neck flexion) (23). Furthermore, Dettmers et al. demonstrated better MI ability for the third-person perspective compared to the first-person in patients after a stroke (24). Reports on patients’ MI perspective preferences were embedded in two further studies. Schuster, Lussi (25) and Wondrusch and Schuster-Amft (26) described that the majority of the included patients with sensorimotor impairments preferred an external MI perspective for imaging gestures, if they were free to choose a MI perspective.
Several factors that seem to influence the choice of a certain MI perspective have been described in the literature. Mulder, Hochstenbach (27) point out the importance of age. Their results showed that healthy elderly participants (> 64 years) have some decline in the ability to imagine movements from the internal perspective. Based on this finding, they suggested assessing MI ability of a participant before starting a MI training program. An age related shift to the external perspective preference was supported by Kalicinski, Kempe (28) as well. Indeed, when MI capacity decreases with age, the therapeutic application of MI in motor rehabilitation becomes uncertain, especially with regard to neurorehabilitation, as the majority of patients belong to the older population (29). Furthermore, Mulder and colleagues discussed a possible association between the physical activity level and MI ability (27). It was proposed that a decreased physical activity level during aging could have a negative influence on the ability to imagine movements, particularly in relation to MI from an internal perspective (27).
There are further indications that healthy individuals as well as patients with sensorimotor impairments might change their MI perspective preference. Jiang, Edwards (30) reported that four out of 15 healthy participants aged between 19 and 29 years could not maintain their imagery perspective during brain scanning while imaging running up stairs. Seiler, Monsma (31) removed two participants from their study because they were switching MI perspectives. White and Hardy (20) had to exclude three out of 24 healthy participants (first-year sport health and physical education students) from their analysis due to the same reason. A change of preferred MI perspective in patients with motor impairments between two measurement sessions was further observed by Schuster, Lussi (32) when seven out of 73 patients changed their preferred MI perspective.
Apart from the MI perspective, the MI user may also choose between a visual and a kinaesthetic modality. The visual modality focuses on the visualisation of the movement, while the kinaesthetic modality focuses on the sensation of the movement (14). Kinaesthetic MI is commonly allocated to formation of an internal MI perspective. Dijkerman, Ietswaart (12) claimed that only an internal MI perspective could accompany kinaesthetic imagery. Jeannerod (33) proposed that internal MI involves kinaesthetic representations, while external MI refers to visual representation of an action. On the other hand, White and Hardy (20) and Callow, Roberts (13) suggested, that it might also be possible to experience kinaesthetic MI with external visual MI. In their study with sport students and healthy young adults, both study groups using either internal imagery or external imagery equally reported kinaesthetic sensation during MI. Unfortunately, MI perspective and MI modality are not always recognised as distinct elements of the technique. While several studies provide information on how participants were instructed to engage in MI, most of studies did not disclose the MI modality used by the participants (34, 35). Conversely, other studies make the distinction between MI modality but do not report the MI perspective (8, 14). Meanwhile there are investigations exploring brain activations ivolving these imagery modalities and perspectives (30, 31). They convey the importance of individual differences in the different conditions.
There seems to be contradictive evidence from regarding different best combinations of MI perspectives and modalities, thus it remains uncertain. To include MI in clinical practice and to develop useful instructions for MI training, it is important to understand the role of MI perspectives in a clinical setting.
Aim, research question, and hypothesis
The main aim of this study was to explore the spontaneous MI perspective selection over a certain time period leading to the following research question: What MI perspective do patients with sensorimotor impairments spontaneously select during visual and kinaesthetic MI over four measurement sessions?
The secondary aim was to evaluate whether MI perspective preference alters with age and if the physical activity level would influence MI perspective preference.
It was hypothesised that participants select an internal or external MI perspective in both visual and kinaesthetic imagery modalities, and that there might be changes of the MI perspective preference over time.
It was assumed that the imagined kind of movement itself could influence MI perspective preference, and that patients ≥ 64 years and being less active patients would prefer an external MI perspective spontaneously.