Attention allocates limited cognitive resources to task-relevant stimuli to enhance task performance 1,2. While attention was once thought to be continuously focused on a selected target 3, recent behavioral and neuroscience research has found that attention is more like a “blinking spotlight” that periodically samples multiple stimuli 4. Behavioral dense sampling and neurophysiological research have found that attention samples at a rate of about 8 Hz when faced with a single target. In contrast, when faced with two targets, the sampling rate changes to about 4 Hz, suggesting that attentional sampling dwells on each target for a relatively fixed amount of time 5–9. However, other studies have found that attentional sampling is flexible and adaptive. When the number of targets is increased to four, the sampling rate is accelerated to ensure a fixed oscillation period and thus maintain perceptual continuity 10. Periodic attentional sampling also accelerates in complex visual search tasks 11. However, tasks in the real world are complex, and in some cases, a single target is selected from multiple candidate stimuli, whereas in other cases, attention must integrate multiple stimuli into a single target for detection. Whether the sampling dynamics of attention are similar in these cases remains unclear.
Neural oscillations in the brain play an important role in the dynamic sampling of attention. The frequency of neural oscillations, especially the individual alpha frequency, represents the rate of attentional sampling 12,13. When the frequency is high, the period of attentional sampling is short, while when the frequency is low, the period of attentional sampling is long. The phase of neural oscillations, particularly in the theta and alpha bands, is thought to play an important role in target selection, distractor suppression, attentional shifts, and saccades 14–16. Multiple stimuli can be processed in different phases of an oscillatory cycle, with task-relevant stimuli being assigned to excitatory phases and task-irrelevant stimuli to inhibitory phases 4,17. The excitatory phases are used for attentional engagement, while the inhibitory phases promote attentional shift and saccade 4,18. The effects of tasks on attentional sampling are also reflected in neural oscillations. Temporal integration/segmentation tasks affect the alpha frequency of neural oscillations and thus, the temporal resolution of attentional sampling 13. In the spatial domain, however, it has not been investigated whether single-stimulus selection and multi-stimulus integration lead to different attentional sampling dynamics and associated neural oscillations.
When investigating the attentional sampling of multiple stimuli, a technical difficulty is to isolate and compare the neural responses to each stimulus from the overall neural responses elicited by simultaneously presented multiple stimuli. The steady-state visual evoked potential (SSVEP) provides an excellent solution to this problem by directly measuring the neural response synchronized with the flicker frequency of the stimulus, thereby isolating the response to different stimuli 19,20. At the same time, SSVEP power is sensitive to attention, and when attention is directed to a particular stimulus, the corresponding SSVEP power is elevated 21,22. Previous SSVEP studies have mostly used low frequencies, which interfere with the prevalent low-frequency oscillations in the brain, such as theta, alpha, and beta oscillations. Therefore, it has been difficult to investigate the interaction between the changes in SSVEP and the endogenous low-frequency oscillations in the brain. In the present study, we used high-frequency SSVEP 23 to tag the presented stimuli to investigate the attentional sampling of multiple stimuli, the task’s effects on the sampling dynamics, and its neural oscillatory mechanism.
In the present study, we asked participants to select one of the stimuli presented on either side of the fixation point as the target or to integrate the two stimuli as a single target, and we tagged the stimuli with high-frequency flicker (42 Hz on the left and 44 Hz on the right). By analyzing the temporal dynamics of the SSVEP envelopes, we investigated the dynamics of attentional sampling over the two stimuli and the effect of the task on them. The results showed that the envelopes of the SSVEPs for both stimuli exhibited fluctuations in the theta band (4.4 Hz and 6.2 Hz), either in the single-stimulus selection task or in the two-stimulus integration task. Critically, the two SSVEP envelopes showed a significant phase difference during the selection task, indicating different temporal sampling for the two stimuli. In contrast, the integration task, which required spatial integration, showed synchronized sampling as reflected by a smaller phase difference between the two envelopes. Furthermore, by analyzing the coupling between SSVEP amplitude and EEG phase, we identified the right frontoparietal areas responsible for modulating these sampling dynamics through phase modulation. Finally, we observed a significant correlation between the observed phase differences and behavioral performance in the integration task. This study identifies a flexible, task-dependent mechanism for attentional sampling, whereby attention aligns different stimuli to be processed at different times when selecting a single stimulus and aligns different stimuli to be processed at nearly same time when integrating multiple stimuli.