In this paper we analyzed eye-tracking data collected during the HCP 7T resting state fMRI scans in a group of monozygous and dizygous twins, which allowed to assess the degree of heritability of these measures. In addition, since multiple resting scans were collected, it was possible to estimate the reliability of the blink and pupil measures. We also investigated the interaction between blink and pupil dynamics that we termed the blink-induced pupillary response (BIPR). Though the association between blink and pupil changes had previously been acknowledged it was typically disregarded as an artefact, whereas here we provide evidence that it is a physiological response that is reliable and heritable.
First, we show that while vigilance states had an influence on the measures, when only subjects with paired Vigilant-Drowsy paired runs were used, the effect of vigilance did not reach significance, meaning that genetic factors and individual variability dominated these measures, reducing potential influences from purely vigilance related factors. In another words, variability explained by vigilance is not as strong as the variability explained by the individual and genetic factors.
We also found that within any given state (i.e., vigilant or drowsy) eye measures were very reliable for the vigilant state (> .812), and less so but still strongly reliable for All Drowsy state (> .615). Interestingly, the temporal measures including D and C peak time, and D – C peak time difference were more reliable than the amplitude measures (see Supplementary Table 2).
MZ intra-pair correlations were higher than the DZ intra-pair correlations, leading us to test for heritability. We found that AE models (additive and environmental factors) explained the variance better than other models. We found that variance was explained by additive factors (A) from moderate to high degrees (.42 – .62) for all the variables except the D-peak amplitude, and the D – C magnitude drop, where the variance explained by A was small or insignificant, and environmental effects were stronger. Particularly, all the BIPR peak-time measures and C-peak amplitude measure emerged as the most important features related to heritability.
Pupil dilations (and constrictions after blink, BIPR) and blinks are two important but segregated and in most of the times neglected physiological responses that are closely linked to vigilance networks [3, 11]. Our study pointed out the possibility that blink and pupillary consequence after blinking might be driven by similar/overlapping neural mechanisms. For instance in humans, electrophysiological responses (EEG and MEG) to pupil dilations and constrictions are found to be such that pupil dilation peaks are associated with posterior alpha and low beta (8-16Hz) synchrony accompanied/followed by an anterior low (delta) frequency (2-4Hz) desynchronization during wakeful rest [5, 13, 29]. Interestingly in healthy individuals blink related EEG oscillations showed parietal-occipital delta/low-alpha synchrony 500ms after blinking, accompanied by alpha (8-12Hz) desynchronization [3, 30, 31]. Pupil changes were also found to be in synch with the activity of a broad range of ascending arousal nuclei [32]. This is similar to our recent findings showing BOLD activation of ascending brain stem arousal nuclei during blinks [11]. Future work detecting neural activity (i.e., BOLD signal) simultaneously during pupil size changes and blinks where blink-independent phasic pupillary changes could be segregated from the blink-related phasic changes is needed to distinguish the neural correlates underlying these peripheral measures of arousal.
Pupil size is influenced both by the sympathetic nervous system through noradrenergic connections via the Suprachiasmatic Nucleus (SCN) and the LC acting over the dilator muscle, which dilates the pupil, and by the parasympathetic system over the Edinger-Westphal nucleus (EW) through cholinergic pathways acting on the sphincter muscle, which constricts the pupil [28]. LC also has inhibitory effects over the EW. If the cholinergic antagonist Tropicamide is used to block ACh on the sphincter muscles, the phasic pupil dilation is diminished and the tonic pupil dilation is delayed [33, 34], while Phenylephrine, an adrenergic a-1 receptor agonist preserved phasic pupil dilation and partially preserved the tonic pupil size [33], indicating that the iris sphincter muscle and the parasympathetic system play a primary role not only in constricting the pupil but also in controlling rapid pupil dilation. Sustaining pupil size on the other hand is the act of both the dilator and sphincter muscles via both cholinergic and noradrenergic systems. Blinks emerge in a very fast manner and the light hitting the retina changes within tens of milliseconds during which arousal systems might find a moment to reset their activity. Blinking can allocate sympathetic and parasympathetic systems balancing the neural synchrony in subcortical and cortical brain regions, in an individual specific and heritable manner.
Lastly, the orientation response [18] may not only be a light reflex, but also part of a system most likely mediated by brainstem nuclei including Superior Colliculus (SC) and Locus Coeruleus (LC), leading to the orienting of covert and overt visual attention, to help maintain arousal levels at satisfactory levels. As mentioned in studies by [9, 35] and emphasized in [36], orienting responses could be externally driven or as in our case, internally driven, such that internal arousal surges initiated by the ascending arousal nuclei generating (or co-incident with) the spontaneous eye-blinks [11] could be part of a system that can be named as the ‘Oculomotor Adaptive System’, OAS, that includes the SC. For instance, the initial pupil dilation after the SC stimulation in Bell et al. study (driven by the sympathetic system) and its timing looks very similar to the D-peak BIPR component. It would be valuable for future studies to provide further neurophysiological evidence about this system and to investigate it across various neuropsychiatric conditions to determine its potential as a biomarker for diagnosis and for predicting or monitoring treatment responses.
Potential constraints
Unit of measure
As units of measure we used the arbitrary units given by EyeLink1000 as reported in the HCP database. Calibration was conducted and the distance measures between the eyes and the camera were registered in the data acquisition computer as default settings by the HCP staff before each experiment reaching good standards.
Head motion and pupil detection
Concerns could be raised about the effects of head movement on pupillary responses. However, because head movements introduce significant artifacts in the MRI measures the experiments require that the head of the participants while lying in the magnet be strictly stabilized, which is achieved by the placement of foams around the head and inside the head coil (something like a helmet). Thus, we are confident that the head movement was minimal in the 7-T MRI setup. We also show that frame-wise displacement (FD) was not significantly different between vigilance status. For the HCP data set the EyeLink did not record the eye video so instead we monitored eye closures when we detected a pupil loss (marked as ‘0.0’ in the file). We detected a few weird cases in the HCP eye tracking raw data (.asc file) such that any ‘0.0’ valued intervals (missing pupil timepoints) that were regarded and labelled as ‘EBLINK’ even though they could be as short as 10ms and as long as 30 seconds or more. That’s why in our analysis we did not rely on their labelling. Instead, we went through the raw data and selected blinks when the pupil value was 0.0 and continued anywhere from 40ms to 400ms. Thus, the eyeblink onset time was selected as the moment when the pupil value turned to 0.0 and stayed zero within this window range.
For pupil detection under partial eyelid, the EyeLink system mainly uses the ellipse-fitting pupil model. This is preferable if the pupil is significantly occluded (for example by the eyelids) as the ellipse fitting algorithm may give a more accurate estimation of pupil position. The ellipse-fitting mode decreases drift potential and copes well with pupil occlusion but at the cost of a higher noise level. Please see https://fchetail.ulb.ac.be/wp-content/uploads/EyeLink-1000-User-Manual-1.5.0.pdf, section ‘Pupil Tracking Algorithm’. We think that partial eyelid closures that are strong enough to block pupil detection even with the elliptical approach under heavy drowsiness status may take much longer than 400ms, leading to micro sleep episodes. In the Supplementary material we present example figures showing original FD (circle represent a value per TR/Volume). Note that movements are extremely small, most probably respiration related head motions. Another figure is also presented with the FDs overlaid on Fig. 1 in the Supplementary Material. Another plot shows the distribution of the correlation values between pupil size and the FD across runs to see whether FD and pupil size are distributed normally (Supplementary Material). We are confident that head motion was not a problem and show with these results that the blinking and head motion measures are independent components.
Lastly, sometimes individual differences in the pupil-cornea color contrast were weak not letting the system detect the corneal reflection and the pupil, and also the use of contact lenses can impact pupil detection and size measure.
Microsleeps
There can be ‘microsleep’ episodes particularly during the drowsy scans. Generally, microsleeps are regarded as eye-closures of more than 4s [37] and initiation of microsleeps can induce wide negative BOLD signals in fMRI, and reduce respiration and heart rate both of which recover back to the initial values as the micro-sleep ends. However, the most reliable measure of detecting sleep is the EEG (Electroencephalography) using alpha, theta and low delta frequency oscillations, detection of K-complexes and spindles, which are sleep stage dependent and part of up- and down-states in sleep. In this dataset there were no EEG recordings. These vigilance fluctuations (and drowsy context) can induce dynamic effects on the pupil size and consecutively on BIPR, which is consistent with the results we show in this manuscript.