This is the first study with a case series design to demonstrate the feasibility and efficacy of Brief Behavioural Treatment for Insomnia for individuals after brain injury on sleep outcomes for all participants, including sleep quality with change evident for the majority of participants on questionnaire measures with accompanying improvement in mood.
More specifically, regarding the feasibility of BBTI in this sample, most participants tolerated the intervention well. The drop-out rate from the start of intervention to post-intervention was 22.22%, which is comparable with other intervention studies conducted in this population (36) as well as BBTI investigations in other groups (20). Further, seven of the nine participants who commenced the intervention attended all sessions and completed all sleep diaries. Moreover, the feedback given in the Treatment Acceptability and Credibility Questionnaire pointed to very high satisfaction with all treatment components, exceeding the already positive ratings BBTI received in other populations (24).
As far as the preliminary efficacy of BBTI in this sample is concerned, five out of seven participants demonstrated clinically significant sleep improvement post-treatment, one participant showed (clinically nonsignificant) response and one participant showed nonresponse, according to the predetermined clinical change criteria. While no universal standard for classifying insomnia treatment response exists (37), other BBTI studies used criteria similar to ours and reported relatively comparable ratios (38). The proportion of participants with brain injury and insomnia demonstrating a clinically significant response to CBT-I reported in Ouellet and Morin (22) (73%) was comparable to our results. This indicates that for people with brain injury a shorter and more easily disseminated intervention, such as the BBTI, may be a good alternative to the better-established CBT-I in terms of efficacy.
Participants in the clinically significant improvement category presented markedly improved changes in both sleep and insomnia questionnaire measures (PSQI and SCI) as well as sleep diary parameters (TWT, SE, SQ). Moreover, the inter-night variability in these parameters, which is typical of insomnia (39) was reduced in most cases. Sleep diary parameter changes were mostly gradual (except for P2) and appeared within one or two weeks after starting the intervention. As this is the same time frame for improvement noted in Ouellet and Morin (22) it is possible that observable positive effects by the second week of treatment may serve as a reliable predictor of the ultimate treatment response. Furthermore, where follow-up data were available, sleep-related improvements were well-maintained, or indeed further augmented, which provides evidence for the durable effects of BBTI already established by previous research (23), however the sample size is too small, in the context of case series methodology, for firm conclusions to be drawn.
Sleep-related improvements were in most cases accompanied by a decline in self-reported anxiety and depression symptoms, mirroring the findings of Fuller et al. (40). The general association of insomnia with mental health complaints is well-documented, yet its directionality remains unclear (41). Consequently, it is difficult to ascertain whether in the present study sleep improvements led to anxiety and depression reductions or reductions in anxiety and depression brought about improvements in sleep or whether each operated relatively independently. This latter option tentatively emerges as most likely, considering the two participants who did not show clinically significant sleep improvements (P5, P7) both presented anxiety and depression symptom reductions.
While these findings are promising overall, the characteristics of the selected design (particularly the lack of a control group) do not permit establishing with certainty that improvements arose due to the administered insomnia treatment. Several alternative explanations must be considered (42). First, if we assumed sleep disturbances spontaneously improved as brain recovery progressed, then the passing of time since injury could have led to the alleviation of reported sleep complaints. Yet, based on the participants’ self-report, spontaneous improvements of sleep disturbances since the injury ranged from non-existent to minimal and the insomnia symptoms had been present for a median of four years. It is thus implausible that the observed marked change occurring over the course of a few weeks could be solely due to the passing of time. Future studies should consider including a control group or implement single case experimental design methodology to increase confidence of any observed changes being due to the effects of the intervention.
Second, while face-to-face client-therapist contact was limited to once per week, participants received daily text reminders which contained some personalised elements. Thus, the virtually daily study contact could have induced the symptom improvements, rather than the BBT-I itself. However, self-reported adherence to particular treatment recommendations typically preceded observed effects. Further, participant 5 who complained of insomnia despite already practicing most of the BBTI recommendations and emphasized liking the regular contact did not show improvement on sleep parameters merely through contact with the therapist. Therefore, despite constituting an important active element to any talking therapy, it is unlikely that contact via text messaging could solely account for the observed treatment effects.
Two participants did not show clinically significant treatment improvements. For P7 (demonstrating “response”), modest positive effects appeared towards the end of the treatment course. P7 presented with further improvements in the weeks following the end of the treatment, reaching the criteria for clinically significant improvement at 1-month and 2-month follow-ups. Notably, for P7 the timing of the treatment coincided with several adverse life events, which may have masked the benefits of the intervention at that time, with these benefits fully appearing when some time since the adverse events has passed.
Only P5 showed “nonresponse” to treatment, albeit presenting a high motivation to change and being one of the only participants with significant family support. In this case, it is plausible that BBT-I failed to produce sleep improvements as the participant was already instinctively practicing most of the core treatment elements. Their total sleep time was already short and any further restriction would have violated the recommended safe minimum. As discussed by Buchanan et al. (24) and in accordance with stepped-care model of insomnia (43), there may be a small subset of clients for whom short and purely behavioural therapy is not sufficient and such clients should be considered for the longer CBT-I delivered by a sleep specialist.
Limitations
Several limitations to the present study must be considered, such as lack of access to medical records. All information regarding brain injury characteristics was obtained through self-report, which could have contained important distortions, especially considering any post-brain-injury memory problems (44). Individuals with brain injury have been shown to overestimate their sleep difficulties when subjective reports were compared to objective data (28, 45). The current sample consisted of participants attending Headway support services hence it is possible that their insomnia symptoms did not impact their social engagement. Additionally, participants’ responses to HADS demonstrate low levels of depression. As depression is highly comorbid with insomnia(46) and has a high prevalence in those following a brain injury (47) future studies should explore whether participants with moderate to severe symptoms of depression may benefit from the intervention.
Future studies should include objective measures of sleep and circadian patterns, such as actigraphy or ambient recorded polysomnography. Lastly, longer follow-ups may have been more informative as to the long-term maintenance of the benefits achieved. Follow up data were not collected for a few participants due to difficulty to maintain contact.
Future directions, implications and conclusions
The promising findings of our study expand on the existing evidence base suggesting that brief, non-specialist-delivered and purely behavioural therapies can yield positive results in people with insomnia after brain injury. Further research is needed to investigate the use of this treatment for insomnia in people after ABI employing single case experimental design and randomised controlled trial protocols.