Table 1
Demographic Statistics for Age and Siblings
|
n
|
%
|
Mean
|
Age Range
|
|
|
21.06
|
17–19
|
165
|
31.0%
|
|
20–22
|
250
|
47.0%
|
|
23–25
|
137
|
25.8%
|
|
26–28
|
14
|
2.6%
|
|
29 and above
|
2
|
0.4%
|
|
Number of Male Siblings
|
|
|
|
1
|
481
|
86.7%
|
|
2
2+
|
44
|
7.9%
|
|
40
|
7.2%
|
Elder/Younger Brother
|
|
|
|
Elder
|
238
|
43.0%
|
|
Younger
|
285
|
51.4%
|
|
Both Elder + Younger
|
44
|
7.9%
|
|
Twin
|
2
|
4.0%
|
|
The majority of participants were between 20 and 22 years old at the time of the study, with a mean age of 21.06 years. More than half of the participants reported having only one brother, typically a younger brother. This finding is in alignment with previous studies conducted in India and other countries, which have documented couples' attempts to conceive a male child after the birth of a first-born female (Singh et al., 2021; Chaudhuri, 2012; Basu & de Jong, 2010; Clark, 2000). Only two participants had a twin brother.
Table 2
Descriptive Statistics of the study variables
Variable
|
N
|
M
|
SD
|
Skewness
|
SE Skewness
|
Kurtosis
|
SE Kurtosis
|
PFGD
|
568
|
16.42
|
5.95
|
-0.15
|
0.10
|
-0.95
|
0.21
|
GT
|
568
|
19.78
|
3.70
|
-0.51
|
0.10
|
-0.15
|
0.21
|
SE
|
568
|
30.28
|
4.91
|
-0.25
|
0.10
|
0.75
|
0.21
|
The mean score for PFGD was 16.42 (SD = 5.95), indicating moderate levels of perceived discrimination within families. GT had a mean score of 19.78 (SD = 3.70), suggesting a moderate degree of gender transcendence among participants. SE scored higher with a mean of 30.28 (SD = 4.91), reflecting generally high self-efficacy among the sample. The sample fulfils the assumptions of normal distribution, and it is neither skewed, nor platykurtic, and nor leptokurtic. This was concluded by finding a common ground among numerous sources that have given different acceptable range of values of skewness and kurtosis. Field (2013), Gravetter & Wallnau (2014), and George and Mallery (2010) proposed a range of ± 2; Tabachnick and Fidell (2013) suggested the threshold between − 1.5 to + 1.5; and Bulmer (1979) recommended the range of ± 1.
Table 3
Correlations among the study variables
|
Mean
|
SD
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
1
|
Perceived Familial Gender Discrimination
|
16.4155
|
5.95455
|
|
− .262**
|
− .223**
|
2
|
Gender Transcendence
|
19.7817
|
3.69921
|
− .262**
|
|
.476**
|
3
|
Self-Efficacy
|
30.2764
|
4.90970
|
− .223**
|
.476**
|
|
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
|
Pearson correlations in Table 3 revealed significant relationships between the variables. PFGD was negatively correlated with gender transcendence (r = -0.262, p < .01), indicating that higher levels of perceived gender discrimination were associated with lower levels of gender transcendence. This proves Hypothesis 1(a), which states that PFGD will have a significant relationship with GT. Dhar, Jain, and Jayachandran’s (2019) study on sixth and seventh graders and their parents from Haryana uncovered a significant positive correlation between parental and child attitudes, with maternal influence surpassing paternal influence. On an average, when a parent holds a more discriminatory gender attitude, their child is 15 percentage points more likely to adopt a similar stance. Notably, mothers' attitudes wield a 50% stronger impact compared to fathers'.
PFGD was negatively correlated with self-efficacy (r = -0.223, p < .01), indicating that higher levels of perceived gender discrimination were associated with lower levels of self-efficacy. This proves Hypothesis 1(b), which states that PFGD will have a significant relationship with SE. Dercon and Singh’s (2013) cohort study delves into the impact of gender bias and discrimination on self-efficacy among children aged 8, 12, and 15 in Ethiopia, India, Peru, and Vietnam. The study comprehensively examines various facets such as nutrition, education, aspirations, subjective well-being, and psychosocial abilities (self-efficacy, trust, self-esteem, and inclusion). The findings show the presence of entrenched gender bias against girls in education, particularly evident in India and, to some extent, in Ethiopia. In particular, girls in these regions exhibited lower levels of self-efficacy. Specifically, the research underscores that self-efficacy is considered as a masculine trait not expected to be possessed by females, within the contexts of India and Ethiopia, a phenomenon directly linked to the prevailing institutionalized gender bias.
Table 4
Regression Analysis Coefficients predicting Self-Efficacy from Perceived Familial Gender Discrimination
Predictor
|
B
|
SE B
|
β
|
t
|
p
|
Constant
|
33.296
|
0.590
|
|
56.45
|
.000
|
PFGD
|
-0.184
|
0.034
|
-0.223
|
-5.445
|
.000
|
GT
|
0.594
|
0.051
|
0.448
|
11.76
|
.000
|
Note: PFGD = Perceived Familial Gender Discrimination; GT = Gender Transcendence.
The dependent variable is Self-Efficacy.
As it can be observed from Table 4, PFGD was a significant negative predictor of SE (B = -0.184, t (566) = -5.445, p < .001), accounting for 5% of the variance in SE (R² = 0.050). This finding suggests that higher perceived gender discrimination within families leads to lower self-efficacy among young women, thus proving Hypothesis 2.
This finding can be explained by what Steele and Aronson (1995) call stereotype threat, in which individuals are at risk of conforming to stereotypes about their social group, which can lead to anxiety and reduced performance in tasks where the stereotype is relevant. A female child’ self-efficacy decreases due to internalized negative stereotypes, anxiety and pressure from her family to conform, restrictions placed by them reducing advanced opportunities to grow and succeed, and lack of support and encouragement by the family itself.
Table 5
Direct, Indirect and Total Effects of the study variables
|
|
|
|
|
Boot 95% CI
|
|
Effects
|
B
|
SE
|
t
|
p
|
LL
|
UL
|
Standardized Coefficent
|
Direct Effects
|
-0.0871
|
0.0314
|
-2.7729
|
.0057
|
-0.1488
|
-0.0254
|
-0.1056
|
Indirect Effects
|
-0.0969
|
0.0185
|
-
|
-
|
-0.1353
|
-0.0631
|
-0.1175
|
Total Effects
|
− .1840
|
.0338
|
-5.4451
|
< .001
|
-0.2503
|
-0.1176
|
-0.2231
|
The mediation analysis revealed that GT partially mediated the relationship between PFGD and SE. The direct effect of PFGD on SE was significant (B = -0.0871, p < .01), as was the indirect effect through GT (B = -0.097, 95% CI [-0.135, -0.063]). This indicates that gender transcendence reduces the negative impact of familial gender discrimination on self-efficacy, though not entirely, thus proving Hypothesis 3. This can be observed in Table 5 and Fig. 1.
Self-discrepancy theory (Higgins, 1987) posits that discrepancies between an individual's actual self, ideal self, and ought self can lead to negative emotions and reduced self-efficacy. Gender discrimination experiences at home creates a self-discrepancy by making the female feel that she does not meet the traditional gender expectations imposed by her family. By adopting non-traditional gender roles, that is, gender transcendence, the young female reduces the internal conflict between societal expectations and personal identity, leading to a partial mediation effect where SE is improved despite the presence of PFGD. Gender transcendence may also serve as a positive cognitive reappraisal method of coping (Gross, 2002), where the individual reinterprets the meaning of traditional gender roles and discrimination, adopting a more egalitarian perspective. This reinterpretation helps to maintain self-efficacy amidst the discriminating home environment. GT also represents a process of empowerment for some females, where the traditional gender roles are transcended, thereby gaining a sense of control and agency. This empowerment (Rappaport, 1987) can partially offset the negative impact of PFGD on self-efficacy by providing the individual with a more robust sense of personal efficacy and enabling the individual to maintain or enhance their self-efficacy despite the adverse effects of perceived familial gender discrimination.