Current estimates indicate that a growing percentage of children in Canada are identified as being neurodiverse, with recent studies indicating approximately 2% of Canadian children are autistic, and 5% of Canadian children are diagnosed with ADHD (Statistics Canada, 2019; Public Health Agency of Canada, 2018; Espinet et al., 2022). While autism and ADHD have distinct diagnostic criteria in the DSM-V, challenges with executive functioning (EF) skills have been seen among both groups (Benallie et al., 2021; Otterman et al., 2019; Carter et al., 2017). EF skills include a broad range of higher order cognitive skills such as response inhibition, self-regulation and mental flexibility (Fuhs, Farran & Nesbitt, 2015; Blijd-Hoogewys, Bezemer & van Geert, 2014). EF abilities play a significant role in the development of academic skills across core subject areas such mathematics, reading, and writing in school-aged children (Cheung & Chan, 2022; Fuhs, Hornburg & McNeil, 2016; Liu, Chung & Fung, 2019; Khng & Ng, 2021; Valcan et al., 2020; Fuhs et al., 2014). This means that neurodiverse children may be more vulnerable to lower academic achievement with increased EF deficits.
Inhibition is an EF skill that involves actively suppressing or delaying action or ignoring unimportant stimuli (Bjorklund & Harnishfeger, 1995; Clark, 1996). This ability becomes increasingly important as children develop, as it is a key skill in goal directed behaviour used within everyday life (Koglin & Petermann, 2004). Challenges with response inhibition have been associated with behaviour and emotional difficulties across various groups of neurodiverse children, including autistic children, preterm-born children, and children with ADHD (Schmitt et al., 2019; Réveillon et al., 2016; López-Martín et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016; Fernades et al., 2016). Studies have shown that both autistic children and those with ADHD have significantly lower response inhibition when compared to neurotypical controls, likely due to neurological alterations primarily in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) (Puiu et al., 2018; Saraçaydın et al., 2024; Gerhardt et al., 2021; Geurts, van den Bergh & Ruzzano, 2014; Kelly et al., 2021). As neurodivergent children are at higher risk for response inhibition difficulties, understanding the environmental risk and resilience factors that can impact the development of these skills is crucial.
Several risk and resilience factors related to response inhibition outcomes have been identified in both neurotypical and neurodiverse children. An increasing prevalent risk factor for the development of response inhibition in both neurodiverse and neurotypical children is excessive screen time. Screen time in children with and without neurodevelopmental disorders increased significantly during the pandemic (Hmidan et al., 2023; Choi et al., 2021, Poitras et al., 2017). Current national guidelines suggest children under 2 years old should not have any screen time, children between 2–4 years old should be limited to 1 hour/day, and children who are 5 years and older should be limited to 2 hours/day of screen time (Ponti, 2023). Concerns regarding excessive screen time use in both neurotypical and neurodivergent children have arisen, as this may be associated with delayed language acquisition (Al Hosani et al., 2023; Dong et al., 2021) and difficulties with executive functioning, including response inhibition (Lin et al., 2020; Soltani Kouhbanani et al., 2023; Law et al., 2023; Efraim et al., 2021).
In order to promote healthy cognitive outcomes in neurotypical and neurodiverse children, two main exploration strategies emerge: 1) preventing risk factors, and 2) promoting resilience factors. Within the literature, it is widely recognized that resilience must be viewed through an ecological framework, considering how internal and external factors interact simultaneously to promote positive outcomes (Rutter, 2006; Avdagic et al., 2020; Ungar, Ghazinour & Richter, 2013). Currently, there is a gap in the literature examining cognitive risk factor.
In our study, we explored how screen time is predictive of response inhibition in distinct groups of neurodivergent children, specifically those with ADHD or ASD. Response inhibition was measured using a Stroop task from a well-established and widely validated online cognitive test battery (Burns et al., 2010). The Stroop task is a valid and reliable method for measuring both response inhibition and working memory (Duell et al., 2018; Sugg & McDonald, 1994). Although online cognitive testing limits some standardization of testing procedures and control over the testing environment, it can increase accessibility to the research study and lower costs, while providing a similar level of accuracy when compared to traditional cognitive testing (Health and Medicine Week, 2023; Tuerk et al., 2023). Using an ecological model of resilience, we aimed to identify common lifestyle factors, which are predictive of performance on a response inhibition task in neurodivergent children.
In a heterogenous cohort of neurodivergent and neurotypical children, we addressed two main aims. The first was to examine differences in lifestyle factors including screen time use, in relation to current national guidelines in different age and neurodiagnostic groups. The second aim was to examine the association between hours of screen time (passive TV watching, social media, video games) and other lifestyle factors (sleep, outdoor time, extracurricular activities, free play, socializing) with performance on the Stroop response inhibition task in relation to diagnosis, adjusting for age and sex. We hypothesized that all children would exceed the amount of recommended screen time based on current national guidelines, and that screen time use would be higher in children with ADHD or ASD. We further predicted that screen time is a risk factor for response inhibition, and that performance on inhibition tasks will have a negative association with screen time for autistic children and children with ADHD.