Our findings revealed significant associations between free memory recall, anxiety, and depression across lockdown group response levels. Individuals in the compliant group had better free recall, those in the sufferers group had significantly greater anxiety, and those in the defiant group had greater depression symptoms during the 2020 period of the global COVID-19 lockdown. Age and gender were also significant predictors of anxiety in the lockdown group. Another important finding was that none of the sociodemographic variables, specifically age and gender, were significant predictors of lockdown group responses and free memory recall relationship. The rationale of this study was to provide insight into the mechanisms of semantic coding associated with free recall memory and how these mechanisms influence varying individual and group compliance responses to pandemic lockdown restriction measures. Additionally, the study showed the intriguing influence of emotional content on the neural processes of individuals and the group’s psychological outlook on social isolation measures.
The significant association between lockdown group responses and free memory recall showed that the comply group exhibited better memory recall than the sufferer and defiant groups. This means that the overall memory recall performance of the individuals in the comply group (those who easily adhered to group/social norms and COVID-19 lockdown rules) indicated a positive recency effect on their delayed recall tasks. This effect tapped into a more stable long-term storage mechanism component of the free recall performance, which is mainly affected by semantic coding33,34. The storage mechanism component is unaffected by the rehearsal-preventing task of delayed recall; as such, correctly recalling texts from the beginning of the passage was more prevalent in the long-term storage component than the short-term storage mechanism of single-trial free recall, which also reflects a recency effect but with immediate recall of the recently rehearsed texts from the end of the passage. It must be noted, however, that, based on the nature of the online survey used in this study, the assumption was that participants engaged the recency effect of delayed recall with distinctively long pauses (between 20 and 30 seconds) that preceded their recall response – a unique response time associated with free recall memory tasks35. In light of this, interpretation of this finding was considered in the context of this observation.
The significant findings across the lockdown group could be attributed to cognitive interpretations of individual/group variations as an indication of significant free memory recall performance findings36. In light of these cognitive variations, it was not unexpected that free memory recall performance between the comply and sufferer groups indicated non-significant differences in memory recall. However, performance significantly differed between the comply and defiant groups and between the sufferer and defiant groups. Another possible reason for this can be attributed to the impact of recent life stress (such as the COVID-19 pandemic) on the cognitive process of the participants. The outcome of this stress, reflected as either acute or chronic, was assumed to impair the retrieval process within the memory recall capacity. As a result, the recall process impinges upon individual episodic memory content, which happens to be the seat of the long-term storage mechanism of the free memory recall task37. This finding indicates that recent life stress, which might be more pronounced in the suffering and defiant groups than in the compliant group in our study, has the potential to limit the capacity of long-term storage mechanisms, especially when individuals or groups are faced with life-threatening psychological and emotional challenges. The COVID-19 pandemic could presumably exert substantial effects on the memory retention and retrieval abilities of individuals with cognitive variations necessary for decision-making responses38.
Age-related decreases in cognitive function could also be attributed to the significant effect of these outcomes. The participants’ sample distribution across different age cohorts showed a positively skewed distribution of their free memory recall scores. Therefore, it was not unusual to expect significant age differences to be associated with the memory recall performance of respondents across lockdown groups in this study. This was prevalent with reference to reduced inefficient recall and retrieval performance of the episodic content of the long-term memory store39. Although evidence-based results have lent support to this assumption40,41, the experimental studies of Craik42 and Raymond43 foreground the assumption that the effectiveness of memory recall from long-term storage of episodic memory is affected by successive age-related increases, including the length and stimulus size of the recalled vocabulary or text44. This finding suggested that there may be certain differential and ineffective cognitive functioning processing in older adults compared to younger adults. This was due to poor retrieval and selective decline in performance from the long-term storage mechanism of episodic memory45.
This study also revealed important significant findings on anxiety and depression across lockdown groups. The results showed that the sufferer group experienced greater anxiety symptoms than the defiant and comply groups. In comparison, the defiant group displayed greater depressive symptoms compared to the sufferer and comply groups. These findings suggested that both anxiety and depressive symptoms were significantly associated across the lockdown group responses. These findings are consistent with previous literature and systematic reviews, especially on COVID-19 behavioural and cognitive responses to social isolation measures10,25,26. While depressive symptoms decreased across the lockdown response levels compared to heightened anxiety levels, the effect sizes of both anxiety (𝜂2 = 0.63) and depression (𝜂2 = 0.50) across group responses were relatively modest. Nevertheless, the significant differences in emotional responses across the lockdown groups could be due to several reasons. First, the global lockdown disrupted people’s personal, financial, and social lives, resulting in a negative psychological outlook. Moreover, the extended ‘sit-at-home’ lockdown restrictions might have elevated individual anxiety and depressive responses. This might have been more prevalent in the sufferer group because of their assumed financial difficulty and low optimism during the pandemic lockdown. This might further aggravate their anxiety levels because of agitation and lower optimism in the government’s response to controlling disease spread.
This explanation is consistent with recent literature on the exacerbated emotional levels of anxiety, stress, and depressive symptoms in people during the COVID-19 pandemic, which has resulted in maladaptive coping responses across individuals and group outcomes36,46. Additionally, the role of misinformation due to high social media exposure and usage, including emerging conspiracy theories on COVID-19 infections and treatments47,48, could also be attributed to significant differences in emotional content across lockdown groups. Frequent social media usage and lack of government plans to respond adequately to changing scientific information (leading to misinformation) might have negatively influenced people’s emotional state toward inappropriate responses to pandemic lockdown measures. For instance, the defiant group, which was susceptible to high social media exposure and usage, was linked to greater depressive symptoms in our study. This finding is consistent with recent studies that postulated the significant association of increasing misinformation, conspiracy theories, and fake news on COVID-19 infections and outcomes with heightened emotional and psychological responses49,50. In light of the reviewed literature and hypothesis that stated that significant differences exist between lockdown group responses to COVID-19 social restriction measures on participants’ emotional content of anxiety and depression, the findings of this study therefore offer consistent support for the body of knowledge in favour of the significant effects of these measures on emotional responses to COVID-19 lockdown measures.
Consistent with the findings of recent studies27,51, our regression analyses showed that age significantly predicted the associations between lockdown group responses and emotional responses to anxiety and depression. The negative prediction of the result suggested that younger adults were more exposed to heightened levels of anxiety and depression than older adults were. An explanation for this could be that older individuals have the emotional capacity or greater memories of overcoming past difficulties, to contextualise a stressor, such as the outcome of the COVID-19 lockdown measures, which helps them maintain a more stable emotional balance than younger individuals52. Additionally, older adults tend to have a more mature social disposition and better financial support base, such as better-paying jobs, which might reduce the context of emotional imbalance than younger adults. Furthermore, gender significantly predicted group responses and anxiety relationship. Compared to males, females showed higher anxiety symptoms, which was consistent with the findings of previous studies indicating a significant increase in anxiety observed in females53,54. A potential explanation for this could be because of the assumptions that females bear more disproportionate domestic and caregiving responsibilities than men, especially during the pandemic period, such as during the COVID-19 social isolation, which results in contextually skewed gender divisions of labour in society, including household settings55,56. As a result, females could be considered more susceptible to increased anxiety and depressive symptoms. Social isolation measures such as restricted physical mobility during the pandemic could also increase females’ exposure to domestic violence and hostile experiences. This could exacerbate their emotional disturbance levels, especially in areas where gender violence practices and narratives are prevalent, such as sub-Saharan Africa, including Southern Africa57–59.