4.1 Cortisol and schizophrenia
Changes in saliva cortisol level before and after stress represent the function of the cortisol stress response and HPA axis. The previous stress-vulnerability model theoretically proposed the relationship between HPA axis function and schizophrenia[1–4], which has been verified in several studies[7]. In this study, although the salivary cortisol level measurements did not differ significantly between patients and controls, the existence of time and diagnosis interactions showed that the trends in the cortisol level change of the two groups differed greatly as the psychological stress challenge task progressed. The patients’ cortisol stress response was not obvious. Although it tended to first rise and then fall, the change was not significant. The overall response was flattened, which was similar to the blunted CAR previously reported in patients with schizophrenia[11–13, 15]. The change trend of salivary cortisol levels in the control group differed greatly from that of the patient group; the salivary cortisol level was clearly high before the task began, and then gradually decreased. The difference in cortisol stress response between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls was clear immediately, confirming the hypothesis that patients with schizophrenia had HPA axis dysfunction.
4.2 Cortisol, stress, and distress intolerance
We chose to measure cortisol stress response, as it better reflects the pathological process than a baseline cortisol level[7].
Prior to the start of the psychological stress challenge task, the baseline cortisol level increased due to the effects of anticipation anxiety. Although our study did not take samples at the same time another day for comparison, a previous study performed this comparison, confirming the existence of the expected anxiety[24]. We found pre-task cortisol levels were significantly higher than cortisol levels after the task in the controls, which was inconsistent with the findings of previous studies in an American population[9]. Cortisol levels represent stress levels, so a high level of cortisol suggests that healthy controls already had significant sensations of stress before the task began. Considering that previous studies were conducted in the Western parts of the world, some differences may have been due to our participants being from a Han Chinese population. Many studies revealed racial differences in cortisol levels[27–30], one study also found that people of different ethnicity have different responses to psychological support under pressure[31]. All these suggest that cortisol and stress-related research may have ethnic differences. But there are no specific conclusions for racial differences in the effects of expected anxiety on cortisol levels yet. This point warrants further study.
The influence of culturally related psychological factors is another possible reason for the differences. Studies have shown that cultural differences between the East and the West affect people’s attitudes toward examinations. In some cultural atmospheres that emphasize examinations, test anxiety is more obvious and common[32, 33]; China has such a cultural atmosphere, and compared with participants in previous Western studies [9, 24], our subjects had a lower task withdrawal rate, which may have also reflected this cultural atmosphere. After learning they would take a test, the healthy controls might have had higher expectations for their performance, which could also have caused severe anticipation anxiety. However, the patients felt less stressed before the task, which is consistent with the apathy and decreased self-expectation caused by schizophrenia.
Cortisol levels in both groups showed a downward trend after the 40-minute rest following the task, which is a normal part of stress response. However, the patients’ cortisol level decreased at a much slower rate. The cortisol level of the patient group before the task was significantly lower than that of the controls, while the cortisol level 40 minutes after the end of the task tended to be higher than that of the controls, indicating the response of the patients’ HPA axis was insensitive or delayed, or that the negative feedback mechanism was damaged and unable to escape from stress. This finding mirrors earlier studies, where some patients showed a significant delay in cortisol stress response[9].
Among the patients, the trends of changes in cortisol levels in distress tolerance and distress-intolerance patients were different. Among patients with distress intolerance, there was an upward trend in cortisol levels during the task, then they dropped more sharply after the task. This may indicate that these patients felt that the pressure during the task greatly increased and exceeded their tolerance threshold, leading them to opt-out. When they decided to give up, most of the stress disappeared, and cortisol levels dropped rapidly. However, patients with distress tolerance may have been insensitive to the stress caused by the task, leading to the indifference of the process for completing the task and a smoother curve in the change of their cortisol levels. As this study was limited by a small sample size of patients with distress tolerance, our conclusions warrant further verification.
4.3 Clinical symptoms and other indicators
We found that cortisol stress response was not significantly correlated to patient symptoms, nor was it correlated with negative emotion scores before the task. Education level and smoking status had no significant effect on cortisol stress response, which is consistent with previous research [9, 10].
Although there are few studies on cortisol stress response in patients with schizophrenia, there are some studies on other cortisol indicators, and the results are inconsistent. Most of them found no significant correlation between cortisol and symptoms[34–36], with similar findings for CHR patients [17, 20]. However, a study of treatment-resistant patients with schizophrenia found that patients’ serum cortisol levels were significantly reduced in the morning after treatment, and, although the correlation was weak, it was related to improvement in PANSS negative symptoms[37]. One study also found that, in chronic schizophrenia, cortisol levels were weakly correlated with the PANSS negative-symptom subscale[38]. Another study found that, after treatment, the increase in cortisol during the day in patients with schizophrenia was relieved, but the blunted CAR did not change[15]. These differences may have been due to varying research methods. Previous studies showed that symptoms may be related to cortisol levels within a certain period, reflecting the daily function of the HPA axis, while cortisol stress response represents the mobilization and coping ability of the HPA axis under stress (which has something in common with the CAR). This may indicate that HPA axis mobilization and abnormal coping ability may be a stable biological indicator that can indicate the disease, will not fluctuate with the severity of symptoms, and will be less affected by other factors. Therefore, it has clinical predictive significance and may even become an endophenotype.
We found that the patients with distress intolerance had more severe clinical symptoms, which also indicates that distress intolerance may represent a state that is more deviated from the healthy state, as distress intolerance in the healthy control group was very rare. However, whether distress intolerance is the result of symptoms or one of the factors that cause obvious symptoms remains to be studied.
4.4 Negative emotions
The difference in NA scores between patients and controls is understandable. The psychological stress challenge task is challenging, requires concentration and patience, and plays annoying stimuli in response to a mistake. This process is obviously unpleasant. The NA score of the controls increased as the task progressed, as expected. However, patients with schizophrenia were already experiencing negative emotions before starting the task. The feeling remained almost unchanged during the task, which indicated the patients with schizophrenia were less satisfied with their daily life or less satisfied with their participation in the test, relatively speaking, the content of the task was not their focus.
4.5 Strengths and limitations
To our knowledge, no large-sample study has previously examined the correlation between cortisol stress response and schizophrenia among the Chinese population. Possible racial differences make our research more meaningful. We collected data of more than 160 cases, providing a sample larger than those of previous studies. However, an even larger sample is needed to establish cortisol stress response as a biomarker for schizophrenia.
Cortisol levels may also be affected by other factors, such as alcohol intake or body weight. These factors were not considered in our study. Because the main focus of our study was the change of cortisol levels under stress, there was a self-control effect, and these factors would have had a limited impact on the interpretation of the results. If complete information could be obtained from the subjects, it would help improve the rigor of our conclusions. Cross-sectional studies cannot rule out differences in individual sensitivity to cortisol. Although there were self-control effects and a relatively large sample size, which can minimize the effect of sensitivity differences on the results, more information on sensitivity is needed.
Data to date have been obtained from cross-sectional studies, but disease and treatment conditions may progress or change. Therefore, long-term follow-up data are required to confirm the stability of cortisol stress response in individual treatment.