Predation on large vertebrate prey was an important behavior present during human evolution (Butynski 1982; Thompson et al. 2019). Hunting for the meat of larger animals gave an energetic input that allowed the maintenance of larger brains, enhanced cooperation, prolonged childhood, and shorter female interbirth intervals, among other human characteristics that were only possible by the energy obtained from consuming meat (Thompson et al. 2019; however, see Barr et al. 2022 for a critical view of carnivory link to human evolution). Other primates also hunt and consume vertebrates opportunistically. Olive baboons (Papio anubis) prey upon at least 3 species of primates, 10 ungulates, 4 rodents, 3 hares, 1 bat, 4 herpetofauna and 6 species of bird (Strum 1975; Sommer et al. 2016). Hamadryas baboons (P. hamadryas) can present a vertebrate predation rate of 2.8/100 hours of observation, preying on dik dik (Madoqua kirki), hares, Guinea fowl, among other preys (Schreier et al. 2019). Yellow baboons (P. cynocephalus) hunt lizards, birds, and gazelles (Hausfater 1976; Rhine 1986). The environment can be an important factor in the amount of predation by primates. Baboons living in close forests present a much lower predation rate than those on open savannahs (Sommer et al. 2016).
Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) also prey on vertebrates, such as colobus monkeys and duikers (Hosaka et al. 2002; Hobaiter et al. 2017). Although predation does not seem essential for chimpanzees, they prey at a relatively high frequency (4.65 kills/100h), and those hunts provide a high protein input (Wrangham and Riss 1990). Chimpanzees at Fongoli use wooden tools as “spears” to hunt for galagos at their hiding places (Pruetz and Bertolani 2007; Pruetz et al. 2015). These chimpanzees were thought until recently to be the only known non-human population that systematically hunts vertebrate prey with tools (Pruetz et al. 2015).
Sometimes, there are sexual and age differences in the frequency and success of predation (e.g., male chimpanzees usually hunt more frequently and successfully than females, Watts and Mitani 2002; Gilby et al. 2010). In gracile capuchins (Cebus capucinus), the frequency and success of squirrel hunting by males and females differed across two sites in Costa Rica. In the site with fewer sex differences, simultaneous hunts of multiple squirrels were more frequent, maybe allowing females to have a higher success (Rose et al. 2003). Sex differences in hunting can be due to distinct energetic balances. Female mammals may need more stable food sources to increase their energetic intake during pregnancy and lactation (McCabe and Fedigan 2007), being averse to the more unreliable sources (e.g., hunt). On the other hand, bigger males in highly sexually dimorphic species can have more energetic costs and be more resilient to energetic intake oscillation than females and may seek challenging to catch prey more often (Key and Ross 1999).
Afro-Eurasian primates are regularly used as models to understand the evolution of human behavior regarding several behaviors, including predation and tool use, as they are phylogenetically closer to humans than Neotropical monkeys (Rose 1978). Neotropical monkeys are separated by 40 Myr of evolution from the human lineage (Lima et al. 2018). Still, one group of Neotropical monkeys presents several characteristics similar to those present during hominid evolution and could be used as an alternative model. Capuchin monkeys (Sapajus and Cebus) are omnivorous primates, consuming plants, arthropods, and vertebrate prey (Izawa 1978; Fragaszy et al. 2004). Although primarily arboreal, some Savannah populations of robust capuchin monkeys (genus Sapajus) are highly terrestrial (Wright et al. 2019), and the same populations are also the ones presenting frequent and, sometimes, highly variable tool use behavior (Ottoni and Izar 2008; Spagnoletti et al. 2011; Falótico and Ottoni 2016; Falótico et al. 2018a).
In dry forests, white-faced capuchins (C. capucinus) hunt several vertebrates preys, such as lizards, frogs, birds, squirrels, coatis, anteaters, and bats (Fedigan 1990; Rose 1997; Rose et al. 2003). Brown tufted capuchins (Sapajus apella) in the Amazonian forest have been reported to prey on small vertebrates (Izawa 1978), and a Sapajus group in an urban park in São Paulo (Brazil) was also observed to hunt for birds and share the meat (Ferreira et al. 2002). Although vertebrate prey is a small part of the capuchin diet, it consists of high-quality food rich in protein and fat (Fedigan 1990).
Those results, however, are primarily for gracile capuchins (Cebus) and groups living in dry forests. Vertebrate predation data for robust capuchins (Sapajus) living in savannah environments are lacking to date; there is only a case report of predation of an adult rock cavie, Kerodon rupestris (Filho et al. 2021), and data on snake predation (Falótico et al. 2018b; Silva et al. 2019). In other primates, groups living in closed forest environments presented lower predation rates than ones living in open savannahs (Sommer et al. 2016). If the same is true for capuchins, it is expected that the capuchins from dry environments present higher predation rates than forest capuchins.
Another interest in Savanah robust capuchins is that they have several wild populations that customarily use tools (Ottoni and Izar 2008) and present a high degree of terrestriality (Visalberghi et al. 2005; Falótico 2011; Wright et al. 2019), making them an alternative model to Afro-Eurasian primates to understand vertebrate predation in a similar environment present during human evolution. Moreover, the population of bearded capuchin monkeys (S. libidinosus) from Serra da Capivara National Park (SCNP) studied here is known to use stick probes to enhance predation of lizards, carpenter bees, and other prey (Falótico and Ottoni 2014), a behavior only known to be customary, so far, in this population, and that can be of interest to understand the use of such tools for hunting. The probes at SCNP have an average length of 27.9 cm and are made of tree branches and sticks. Although some probes can be used without modification, most of them (64%) are modified by the capuchins before or during the use (Falótico and Ottoni 2014). The rate of probe tool use in SCNP is 0.27 event/h, and the use is almost only performed by males (97%, Falótico and Ottoni 2014).
I present here data collected for 2 years on vertebrate predation and consumption by two groups of capuchin monkeys in a dry Savannah environment (Caatinga) in Brazil. This paper aims to describe the targets and frequency of predation observed in those groups and explore possible differences between sex and age classes. Adult males are the most common sex-age class observed preying vertebrates in several primate species, including capuchin and other primates (Butynski 1982; Rose 1997). Adult male capuchins are larger than females, thus having more possibilities to predate larger vertebrate prey. Also, females are expected to seek for more stable food sources to cope with reproductive costs fluctuations, being, in theory, less prone to look for unpredictable food sources such as vertebrate prey. Thus, I expect this age-sex pattern in predation also to happen here.