Self-report data
The rating data revealed an overall successful subjective induction of threat anticipation (see Figure 3). For the valence ratings, neither the two contexts nor the two time points T0 (baseline) and T1 (follow-up) were rated differently, Time F(1,47) = .38, p = .54, ηp2 = .01, Context F(1,47) = 1.35, p = .25, ηp2 = .03. A significant Time × Context interaction showed, however, that the safety context was perceived as more pleasant than the threat context after threat induction but not at baseline, Time × Context F(1,47) = 29.39, p < .001, ηp2 = .39. The overall level of arousal was higher during T1 than during T0, Time F(1,47) = 10.03, p < 0.01, ηp2 = .18. While in general the contexts did not differ, Context F(1,47) = 2.32, p = .13, ηp2 = .05, post-hoc tests revealed that, after the threat induction, the threat relative to safety context was rated more arousing, Time × Context F(1,47) = 27.18, p < .001, ηp2 = .37. Threat levels increased over time, Time F(1,47) = 20.98, p < .001, ηp2 = .31 and in intensity, Context F(1,48) = 4.10, p = .05, ηp2 = .08, with post-hoc tests showing that the threat context was perceived as more threatening than the safe context only during T0 and not during T1, Time × Context F(1,47) = 26.22, p < .001, ηp2 = .36. There were no covariation effects with questionnaire scores on any of these measures.
Behavioral data
Table 1 contains the parameter estimates of the hierarchical MPT. Using Bayesian modelling, a priori distribution of the parameters is used before analyses and compared to a posterior distribution which is based on the data using Baye’s theorem. The Bayesian confidence intervals (BCIs) explains within which range the true parameter lies in the posterior distribution, given data and prior distribution. Overlapping BCIs indicate non-significant differences between parameter estimates. While participants were moderately able to recognize items as old or new, it did not matter whether they had seen them in a threatening or a safe context during encoding. This is indicated by the overlapping BCIs of the DT and DS parameters.
Moreover, source recognition was poor (dT = .04 and dS = .01 with large Bayesian confidence intervals for the threatening source [.00 - .39]) and did not differ between a threatening or safe source from encoding, evident in overlapping BCIs of the dt and ds parameters. The guessing parameters reflected the probabilities of the events for source guessing (g = .48 with half of the faces from a threatening and half of the faces from a safe context). The probability of a face having been presented before during encoding compared to new was 2:1. There was a slight conservative guessing tendency of classifying an old face as a face being new (b = .58 with the actual probability of a face being old of .67). No significant associations emerged between parameter estimates and questionnaire scores, which were entered as covariates into the model. Model fit (Klauer’s [42] test statistics T1 p = .25) was good.
Using conventional recognition parameters, mean hit rates and false alarm rates for the threatening context were HR Mthreat = .76 (SD = .15), FAR Mthreat = .20 (SD = .17) and for the safe context HR Msafe = .73 (SD = .16) and Msafe = .20 (SD = .15). Item recognition (HR – FAR) did not differ based on Context, F < 1, p = .40, ηp2 = .02, BFincl = 0.305. Likewise, source identification did not differ significantly as a function of context conditions, Context F < 1, p = .50, ηp2 = .01, BFincl = 0.290 (Mthreat = .21 [SD = .07], Msafe = .22 [SD = .05]). This means that it is 3.28 and 3.45 times more likely that there is no difference between the two context conditions for both behavioral measures. None of the questionnaire measures had an impact on memory performance, Fs < 1, ps > .73.
Table 1. Mean parameter estimates of the latent-trait MPT model for the recognition performance of the item/source memory task.
Parameter
|
M [95 % BCI]
|
SD
|
g
|
0.48 [0.45 – 0.52]
|
0.02
|
b
|
0.58 [0.49 – 0.67]
|
0.05
|
dT
|
0.04 [0.00 – 0.39]
|
0.11
|
DT
|
0.28 [0.05 – 0.48]
|
0.11
|
dS
|
0.01 [0.00 – 0.04]
|
0.02
|
DS = DN
|
0.41 [0.32 – 0.49]
|
0.04
|
Note. For the group-level estimates, posterior means (and SDs) are shown. BCI = Bayesian confidence interval. DT, DS and DN =face recognition parameters, dT, dS = context memory parameters and b, g = guessing probabilities.
Startle reflex
As expected for the encoding phase, the startle reflex was potentiated for faces presented with the threatening compared to the safe background, Context F(1,45) = 37.20, p < .001, ηp2 = .45 (see Figure 4A). A marginally significant Context × Depression interaction indicated that the threat-potentiated startle was more pronounced for more depressed participants, F(1, 41) = 3.76, p = .06, ηp2 = .08. No significant interaction emerged for other questionnaire measures.
Regarding the recognition phase, no difference in startle potentiation was found, Context F(2,70) < 1, p = .64, ηp2 = .01, BFincl = 0.133, making the null hypotheses 7.52 times more likely than the alternative hypothesis. Individual differences based on the questionnaire scores did not modulate the findings.
Skin conductance responses to startle probes
Skin conductance responses to startle probes were more pronounced during the first relative to the second half of the encoding session, Block F(1,43) = 31,43), p < .001, ηp2 = .42. No main effect of Context emerged, F < 1, p = .53, ηp2 = .01, however a significant interaction Context × Block, F(1,43) = 6.56, p < .05, ηp2 = .13. Post-hoc tests revealed that the threatening context elicited a more pronounced SCR compared to the safe context only during the second half of the encoding session, F(1,43) = 6.83, p < .05, ηp2 = .14. Moreover, (marginally) significant interactions emerged for Context with depression, trait-anxiety, and social anxiety, Fs(1, 40) = 4.01, 4.93, and 6.02, ps = .05, and < .05, ηp2 = .09, .11, and .13. These interactions indicate more pronounced differentiation between threat and safety context with higher depression and anxiety scores.
For the recognition phase, startle locked SCRs were not modulated by Context, F < 1, p = .45, ηp2 = .03, BFincl = 0.074, or Block F(1,28) = 3.21, p = .08, ηp2 = .10, BFincl = 0.995. There was also no significant Context × Block interaction, F < 1, p = .45, ηp2 = .03, BFincl = 0.04, making the respective null hypotheses 13.51, 1.01, and 25 times more likely than the alternative hypotheses.
Skin conductance responses to picture onset
During the encoding session, SCRs locked to picture onset varied as a function of Context, F(1,43) = 29.93, p < .001, ηp2 = .41, and Block, F(1,43) = 60.55, p < .001, ηp2 = .59, indicating more pronounced SCRs during threat compared to safety, and during the first compared to the second half of the encoding session (see Figure 4B). There was no significant interaction Context × Block, F(1,43) = 2.10, p = .16, ηp2 = .05, nor any interactions with the questionnaire measures, Fs < 1, ps > .43.
During recognition, SCRs elicited did also not vary between threat and safety, Context, F(2,72) = 1.05, p = .35, ηp2 = .03, BFincl = 0.067, nor between first and second phase of the recognition session, Block F < 1, p = .83, ηp2 = .00, BFincl = 0.106. The Context × Block interaction was not significant, F(2,72) = 1.53, p = .22, ηp2 = .04, BFincl = 0.011. Thus, the respective null hypotheses were 14.93, 9.43, and 90.91 times more likely than the alternative hypothesis. Taken together, in the recognition phase, there was no indication that autonomic arousal was enhanced for faces that had been previously presented with a threatening compared to a safety context. There were no significant influences of the questionnaire measures, Fs < 1, ps > .51.