The use of the boars in this study was approved by Virginia Tech Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, protocol #20–206. The study was carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines. The study was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Animals And Housing
Sixty-five day-old boars (York × Landrace) originated from 11 litters born at the Virginia Tech Swine Center. Litters were farrowed in batches, with two litters in March 2021, three litters in April 2021, and six litters in August 2021. The sows were moved into conventional farrowing crates 3–6 days before the first sow in the batch was due to farrow.
On day 1 of age, boars were selected based on the number of boars in a litter (≥ 6) after which they were weighed and ear tagged. If there were more than six boars in a litter, the heaviest six males were included in the study. On day 1 and at weaning, males were administered 1 ml of CIRCUMVENT® PCV-M G2 vaccine (Merck Animal Health, Omaha, Nebraska, USA) through intermuscular injection. Males were administered 2 ml of Iron Hydrogenated Dextran (VetOne, Boise, Idaho, USA) through intermuscular injection on day 1 and were vaccinated with 2 ml of Rhini Shield TX4 (Elanco, Fort Dodge, Iowa USA) at weaning.
Sows and litters were housed in conventional farrowing crates (2.4 m × 1.5 m) until boars were weaned at (mean ± SD) 19.8 ± 1.4 days of age. Sows were fed 3 kg of a standard commercial lactation diet once per day prior to farrowing and twice per day after farrowing. While in the farrowing crate, piglets had continuous access to a water nipple and a heating mat until weaning. Piglets also had access to a commercial creep feed starting 7 days prior to weaning.
After weaning, males from the same farrowing batch were housed together in wire-floored nursery pens (2.4 m × 1.2 m) containing a nipple drinker and a feeder, at a stocking density of 3–4 piglets/m2. Pigs had ad libitum access to a commercial nursery diet. After (min-max) 25–31 days in the nursery, males were moved to grower pens. Males from the same farrowing batch were housed together in partially-slatted concrete grower pens (2.6 m × 3.0 m) with two nipple drinkers and a feeder, at a stocking density of 1–2 pigs/m2. Pigs had ad libitum access to a commercial grower diet.
In the third farrowing batch, tail biting was observed at 44–46 days of age. All piglets with tail lesions were treated with 0.5 ml of ceftiofur crystalline-free acid (Excede®, Zoetis, Kalamazoo, MI, USA). Thereafter, all nursery pens received two Kong toys (Kong Company, Golden, Colorado, USA), and a herding ball (Jolly Pets, Streetsboro, OH, USA). Thereafter, all grower pens were equipped with a suspended plastic teething ring (QC Supply LLC, Schuyler, Nebraska, USA) along with the two Kong toys and ball.
Treatments
This study included three treatment groups, with males undergoing surgical castration (negative control), males receiving analgesia prior to and after surgical castration (pain relief), and males sham-handled and not castrated (positive control).
Piglets were randomly allocated to one of three treatment groups within a litter using their identification number on day 1. On day 3 of age, the six boars from a single litter were removed from the farrowing crate and placed in a holding cart. A single trained technician performed all castration treatments and handling. Piglets were arbitrarily chosen from the holding cart prior to treatment, castrated or handled, and returned to cart. Once all boars from a single litter were treated after which, piglets returned to the farrowing crate.
In the CAS treatment, individual boars (n = 22) were restrained between the technician’s legs. Piglets were surgically castrated by making two vertical incisions on the scrotum and cutting the spermatic cord with a scalpel blade. These piglets were surgically castrated without anesthesia or analgesia, as is common in the U.S. swine industry 4. The CAS procedure took (mean ± SD) 31 ± 4 sec. Piglets in the CAS-A treatment (n = 21) were surgically castrated as in the CAS treatment, with additional analgesics. A topical lidocaine spray (Sanifo Company, Chattanooga, Tennessee, US) was applied, and the technician waited 5 sec prior to the incisions on the scrotum. The CAS-A procedure took (mean ± SD) 32 ± 4 sec. Following castration, the CAS-A piglets were administered flunixin (VetOne, Boise, Idaho, USA) at 2.2 mg/kg once a day for three days. Piglets in the SHAM treatment (n = 22) were handled similarly as in the CAS treatment; piglets were restrained between the technician’s legs for 30 sec. The SHAM procedure took (mean ± SD) 30 ± 0 sec.
Measurements
The project timeline is provided in Supplementary Figure S1. In short, data were collected on body weight, activity, pain expression, and affective state, including anxiety.
Body Weight
Individual body weights were recorded at day 1, day 3, weaning (day 20 ± 1 of age), and the last day of the trial (day 84 ± 3 of age) for all piglets.
Activity
On day 2 of age, accelerometers (HOBO Pendant® G Data Logger, Onset Computer Corporation, Bourne, MA, USA) were placed on the piglets’ back and secured with vet wrap (Andover Healthcare, Salisbury, MA, USA) around their torso and front legs. The accelerometers were programmed to start recording the following day (day 3 of age) to allow for habituation to the vet wrap and accelerometer prior to data collection. Activity data while in the farrowing crate (home pen) were collected on the X (forward activity) and Z (vertical activity) axes in m/s2 at intervals of 15 sec for 1 hour prior to castration treatments and 1, 6, and 24 hours after castration treatments, resulting in 4 hours of activity data and 960 data points per piglet. In addition, accelerometers were used to record activity during the 3-minute attention bias test at week 1 and week 12 of age. On day 6 of age, the vet wrap and accelerometers were removed. For the piglets tested at week 12 of age, vet wrap and accelerometers were reattached prior to the attention bias test.
Pain Expression and Affective States
Within 1 hour after treatment, and again after 6 and 24 hours, the grimace (indicative of pain expression39), and tail posture and tail motion (indicative of affective state36) were video recorded (EOS Rebel T7 DSLR Camera, Canon, Tokyo, Japan) while piglets remained in the farrowing crate with the sow. If any of the treated piglets were sleeping, the sow was gently stimulated to stand or offered some feed in order to arouse the piglets. Data were not collected from piglets that were nursing or continued to sleep at that specific time point. From the video recordings, piglets were identified and time points were noted in which grimace or tail were clearly visibly for scoring. All observers were blinded for castration treatment. A total of 165 grimaces and 176 tail postures and motions were scored (Table 3).
Table 3. Sample size for piglet face captures from video for the piglet grimace scale scoring and for piglet tail posture and motion captures for affective state assessment 0, 6, and 24 hours after treatment in surgically castrated pigs (CAS), castrated pigs that received analgesia (CAS-A), and sham-handled pigs (SHAM).
Timepoint
|
Piglet grimace capture (n)
|
Tail posture and motion capture (n)
|
Treatment
|
Total
|
Treatment
|
Total
|
CAS
|
CAS-A
|
SHAM
|
CAS
|
CAS-A
|
SHAM
|
0 hours
|
19
|
17
|
18
|
54
|
19
|
20
|
20
|
59
|
6 hours
|
19
|
18
|
18
|
55
|
21
|
18
|
21
|
60
|
24 hours
|
19
|
18
|
19
|
56
|
22
|
18
|
17
|
57
|
Total
|
57
|
53
|
55
|
165
|
62
|
56
|
58
|
176
|
[Table 3]
Piglet pain expression was scored by two observers using the piglet grimace scale (PGS)39, which consists of a 0–6 categorical scoring system with scores of 0–1 representing a piglet experiencing “no-to-low pain” and scores of 4–6 representing “moderate-to-high pain”32,39. Inter-rater agreement was tested for 14 piglet grimaces that were not included in this study, and the agreement was strong among observers (Cronbach’s α = 0.7531).
Affective states were assessed from tail postures and motions using a modified method described by Camerlink & Ursinus (2020)36. Piglets’ tails were kept intact, thus were not docked. Seven tail postures (rather than eight in 36) and five types of tail motion36 were recorded and represented varying combinations of arousal and emotional valence (Supplementary Table 1; Q1: high arousal, positive valence; Q2: low arousal, positive valence; Q3: low arousal, negative valence; Q4: high arousal, negative valence36). Two trained observers showed an excellent inter-rater agreement (Cronbach’s α = 0.9379) based on scores for 25 piglet tail postures not included in this study, and both observers scored all pigs. Two other trained observers showed an excellent inter-rater agreement (Cronbach’s α = 0.9028) based on scores for 20 piglet tail motions not included in this study, and both observers scored all pigs.
Anxiety
At week 1 and week 12 of age, the piglets’ (n = 61) attention bias was determined as an indicator for anxiety in a test modified from9. Castration treatments were balanced across the 1-week-old test group (n = 32) and the 12-week-old test group (n = 29).
A 3 m × 3 m × 1 m (length × width × height) arena with a 1 m × 0.6 m start box was constructed from plywood, placed on a concrete floor in a separate building from where the farrowing crates or pens were located (Supplementary Figure S2).
First, pigs were allowed to habituate to the arena during three sessions, three days prior to the test (days 4–6 of age for WK1 piglets and days 77–89 of age for WK12 piglets). At 4 and 5 days of age, all three WK1 littermates were placed in the arena together for 3 min. At 6 days of age, WK1 piglets were individually placed in the arena for 3 min. Similarly, WK12 pigs from the same grower pen were placed in the arena for 3 min in the first two habituation sessions, and individually in the last habituation session (also outlined online in Supplementary Fig S1). During habituation, no threat or feed were presented.
A day after the third habituation session, pigs’ attention bias was individually tested for 3 min. Testing order was randomly predetermined based on piglet identification number within a farrowing batch. At day of testing, the test arena contained a familiar metal feeding pan located in the center of the arena with an electrolyte solution (Gatorade, The Gatorade Company, Chicago, IL, USA) in week 1 and a mixture of raisins, chocolate chips and apple slices in week 12. Gatorade is a non-carbonated sports drink with water and sugar as the main ingredients and is known to be voluntarily ingested by piglets at pre-weaning ages 40. The threat consisted of a guillotine plywood door that was repeatedly opened and closed loudly (aversive sound repeated approximately 9 times) and a strobe light shining across the arena (aversive light) for 10 sec at floor level. This threat produced flashing lights, squeaking noises, and loud bangs.
During the attention bias test, the piglet was placed in the start box, and after approximately 10 sec the door was opened and the piglet was allowed to exit the start box and enter the test arena. Once the start box door was closed, the test started. The threat was presented after the piglet looked at the guillotine door (Supplementary Fig. S2) or after 10 sec passed. During and after the threat, behavior was recorded live and from video recordings. Live observations included the latency to eat (sec) and whether the piglet ate (yes/no), frequency of urination and defecation, frequency of escape behavior, frequency of vigilance, frequency of attention to threat, and frequency of vocalizations including barks, grunts, and high-pitched vocalizations as described by 9 (Supplementary Table 2). One observer recorded frequency of attention to threat, frequency of urination and defecation, and latency to eat (sec). A second observer recorded frequency of escape behavior and frequency of vigilance. A third observer recorded the frequency of all vocalizations. All observers were blinded to treatments.
Video-based behavioral observations were performed to determine behavioral durations (sec) and frequencies (% of total observations; Supplementary Table 2). Two trained observers recorded these behaviors and were blinded to the treatments. The observers showed an excellent inter-rater agreement (Cronbach’s α = 0.9981).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted using JMP Pro 16 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Pigs were considered the experimental units. Data residuals were assessed for normality using normal quantile plots. Residuals for exploring the duration and frequency of attention to the threat showed normal distribution. Residuals for body weights, all other behavioral durations and frequencies, all activity data, number of piglets that ate, tail posture and motion, and all vocalizations did not show a normal distribution. Even though residuals for those data were not normally distributed, the use of mixed-effects models is appropriate as these are robust to quite severe violations of model assumptions such as the residuals’ distribution41, and allow for more complex models, including random effects. Statistical models are detailed in Table 4.
Table 4
Statistical models applied for each response variable with fixed and random factors included in the analysis.
Response variable
|
Model
|
Fixed
|
Random
|
Body weights
|
Mixed
|
Treatment, age, interaction
|
Piglet identification
|
Activity during the attention bias test
|
Mixed
|
Treatment (by week)
|
Piglet identification
|
Attention bias behavior (duration/latency)
|
Mixed
|
Treatment (by week)
|
Piglet identification, farrowing batch
|
Attention bias behavior (frequency/number of piglets)
|
Ordinal logistic
|
Treatment, test week, interaction
|
|
Grimace scale scores, tail postures, tail motions
|
Ordinal logistic
|
treatment, time since castration, interaction
|
|
[Table 4]
Interaction effects at P < 0.10 were removed from the models. Post-hoc comparisons were performed with Tukey HSD corrections for effects with P < 0.10. Escape, laying down, tail circling, WK1 high-pitched vocalizations, and barks were rarely observed and, therefore, not analyzed (frequency of 7 or lower). Data are presented as LSmeans ± SEM unless otherwise noted.