We successfully recorded LFP data from all 13 participants during the study period. In five participants, LFPs were recorded from both hemispheres, resulting in a total of 18 hemispheres of recording. Demographic and clinical information is summarized in Table 1. In most cases (15 out of 18), the largest LFP peak frequency occurred in the beta range (average peak frequency 17.8 ± 3.7 Hz), but for some subjects (three out of 18) the largest peak was found at lower frequencies in the alpha range (average peak frequency 9.4 ± 1.1 Hz). Stimulation parameters and sensing frequencies are provided in Table 1. Data were collected for an average of 14.7 ± 4.2 days.
Table 1
Participant demographics and historical characteristics
Participant ID | Age (years) | Gender | Disease Duration (years) | PD Subtype | LED (mg) | H&Ya Stage | Survey Results |
PSQI | ESS | FSS | MSQ, q1 | PDSS-2 |
1 | 67 | M | 14 | PIGDb | 625 | 3 | 9 | 22 | 6.67 | Y | 32 |
2 | 71 | M | 14 | PIGD | 900 | 2 | 5 | 15 | 2.89 | - | 6 |
3 | 71 | M | 7 | TDc | 450 | 2 | 10 | 2 | 2.78 | - | 17 |
4 | 65 | F | 21 | PIGD | 750 | 2 | 10 | 14 | 6.33 | Y | 28 |
5 | 60 | F | 14 | TD | 1100 | 2 | 7 | 5 | 2.89 | Y | 13 |
6 | 66 | M | 16 | PIGD | 300 | 3 | 18 | 4 | 5.89 | Y | 36 |
7 | 59 | M | 9 | PIGD | 520 | 2 | 10 | 13 | 2.44 | - | 14 |
8 | 71 | M | 18 | PIGD | 348 | 3 | 6 | 14 | 2.89 | N | 13 |
9 | 71 | F | 21 | PIGD | 150 | 3 | 6 | 4 | 2.44 | Y | 18 |
10 | 78 | M | 14 | PIGD | 500 | 2 | 8 | 4 | 3.67 | Y | 23 |
11 | 63 | M | 11 | TD | 1375 | 2 | 5 | 16 | 5 | Y | 12 |
12 | 65 | M | 10 | PIGD | 1200 | 2 | 7 | 7 | 1.33 | Y | 16 |
13 | 46 | M | 12 | PIGD | 1203 | 2 | 3 | 16 | 5.33 | N | 11 |
aH&Y: Hoehn and Yahr |
bPIGD: postural instability and gait disturbance |
cTD: tremor dominant |
Data from a representative subject are shown in Fig. 1, including long-term beta power measurement overlaid with sleep and wake times derived from actigraphy, as well as activity counts, results of cross-correlation analyses, LFP power spectra obtained at the time of DBS event marking, and probability densities for different LFP frequencies during these events. Clear circadian fluctuation in beta frequency LFP power is seen and is strongly linked to activity state as defined by actigraphy as well as self-recorded going-to-bed and waking-up times. Beta power is consistently higher during wakefulness and reduced during sleep. LFP spectra obtained at bed and wake times show higher beta power at the time of waking than of going to bed. For the subject whose data is shown in Fig. 1, there is a statistically significant difference in scaled beta frequency LFP power between going-to-bed and waking-up, while this difference in other frequency bands was not significant. However, this does not hold true across all subjects.
Although there is significant variability both between subjects and between individual nights for each subject, all subjects had an observable difference in beta frequency LFP power between wakefulness and sleep. Figure 2A shows beta power for all subjects separated by arousal state, as well as results of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test for each subject. We also calculated the degree of overlap in beta LFP power between wakefulness and sleep. A histogram from a representative subject showing the probability densities for LFP power separated by arousal state is shown in Fig. 2B. Overall, there was little overlap in LFP power between wakefulness and sleep, with all but one recorded hemisphere showing less than 25% overlap (Fig. 2C).
To examine whether the variability in LFP signals between subjects was associated with identifiable electrophysiological or subjective clinical attributes, we compared the scaled difference in beta LFP power between wakefulness and sleep against several parameters. There was a statistically significant positive correlation between the value of the beta frequency peak (Hz) recorded and the scaled LFP difference between sleep and wakefulness (Fig. 3A, r = 0.602, p = 0.008). That is, participants with an LFP peak in the higher range of beta displayed a larger difference in the LFP power at this frequency between wakefulness and sleep. We also found a statistically significant negative correlation between average stimulation amplitude (mA) used and the difference in LFP power between sleep and wakefulness (Fig. 3B, r = -0.513, p = 0.029). There was a statistically significant negative correlation between the sample lag between actigraphy and LFP data and the scaled LFP difference between sleep and wakefulness (r = -0.605, p = 0.017, not shown in figure). That is, the larger the difference in LFP power between sleep and wakefulness, the better the alignment between actigraphy and LFP data. We believe this to be a measure of the reliability of data collection, as those participants with a large difference in LFP power between wakefulness and sleep (i.e., those in whom the underlying physiology seems to be captured most faithfully) showed very little lag between actigraphy and LFP data.
Figure 4 shows scaled LFP power across canonical frequency bands, which was captured using the Percept PC’s ‘Events’ function. Recordings which contained significant ECG artifact were not included in this analysis. In contrast to the data from a single participant shown in Fig. 1, where a statistically significant difference in LFP power between going-to-bed and waking-up was seen in the beta frequency range but not in any other frequency band, LFP power was not significantly different between going-to-bed and waking-up in any frequency band when averaged for all participants (Fig. 4A). We also compared LFP power across frequency bands for individual subjects, which is shown in Fig. 4B. In four of 12 hemispheres which did not have significant ECG artifact, beta LFP power was significantly higher at the time of going to bed than waking, while in three hemispheres beta power was significantly lower at the time of going to bed compared to waking. In five hemispheres, the difference was not statistically significant.
To assess the validity of our two time series data, we performed a dynamic time warping analysis. Figure 5 shows the minimum distances between LFP and actigraphy data, separated by activity state (wakefulness vs. sleep) for individual epochs across all subjects (Fig. 5A) and as an average for each participant over the recording period (Fig. 5B, p = 5.8 × 10− 6). In both cases, the distances were larger and there was much more variability in the difference between LFP and actigraphy data during wakefulness than during sleep.
In subject 11, stimulation parameters were changed part way through the study period. This was done due to feelings of anxiety, irritability, and emotional lability that had newly developed after initial DBS programming and enrollment in the study. Decreasing stimulation amplitude did not alleviate these symptoms. He was asked to wean and then discontinue taking methylphenidate, which had been prescribed previously for fatigue.40 Subsequently, BrainSense survey performed 10 days after initial programming revealed more prominent beta peaks in both hemispheres than had been seen previously, with the highest power in contact 2 in both the left and right STN. These changes improved his mood and personality changes within days. Initial and subsequent stimulation settings and sensing parameters are found in Table 2, and further details of this case have been described elsewhere.40 In this subject, LFP recordings demonstrated consistent fluctuations both before and after the change in parameters. These data are shown in Fig. 6. Clear circadian fluctuations in beta frequency LFP power were seen both before and after the change in stimulation, though overall higher beta power was seen after the change, particularly during wakefulness (Fig. 6B, 6C). The proportion of variance in LFP power explained by the time of day in left STN was 0.53 before parameter change and 0.70 after parameter change, and in the right STN was 0.65 before parameter change and 0.70 after parameter change.
Table 2
DBS historical characteristics and LFP recording settings
Participant ID | Recorded hemisphere | DBS Duration (years) | Lead model | Stimulation settings | Sensing contacts | Sensing frequency (Hz) |
1 | Right | 8 | 3389 | C + 1-, 3.0mA/130µs/90Hz | 0, 2 | 10.7 |
2 | Left | 5 | 3389 | C + 1-, 3.2mA/60µs/130Hz | 0, 2 | 21.5 |
3 | Left | 7 | 3389 | C + 1-, 2.0mA/60µs/140Hz | 0, 2 | 20.5 |
4 | Right | 8 | 3389 | C + 2-, 2.6mA/100µs/60Hz | 1, 3 | 24.4 |
5 | Right | 8 | 3389 | C + 2-, 3.1mA/70µs/180Hz | 1, 3 | 13.7 |
6 | Right | 12 | 3389 | C + 1-, 2.0mA/270µs/90Hz | 0, 2 | 12.7 |
7 | Bilateral | R: 5 L: 5 | 3389 | R: C + 1-, 3.8mA/60µs/130Hz L: C + 2-, 4.4mA/60µs/130Hz | R: 0, 2 L: 1, 3 | R: 15.6 L: 8.8 |
8 | Left | 16 | 3389 | C + 1-, 3.4mA/60µs/130Hz | 0, 2 | 14.7 |
9 | Bilateral | R: 12 L: 12 | 3389 | R: C + 2-, 6.0mA/60µs/130Hz L: C + 2-, 4.6mA/90µs/130Hz | R: 1, 3 L: 1, 3 | R: 14.7 L: 8.8 |
10 | Right | 7 | 3389 | C + 2-, 2.6mA/60µs/135Hz | 1, 3 | 19.5 |
11 | Bilateral | R: <1 L: <1 | B33005 | R: C + 1-, 2.0mA/60µs/130Hz L: C + 1-, 2.5mA/60µs/130Hz | R: 0, 2 L: 0, 2 | R: 20.5 L: 21.5 |
| | | B33005 | R: C + 2ABC-, 2.0mA/60µs/125Hz L: C + 2ABC-, 1.5mA/60µs/125Hz | R: 1, 3 L: 1, 3 | R: 19.5 L: 21.5 |
12 | Bilateral | R: <1 L: <1 | B33005 | R: C + 2ABC-, 1.8mA/60µs/125Hz L: C + 1ABC-, 1.0mA/60µs/125Hz | R: 1, 3 L: 0, 2 | R: 14.7 L: 13.7 |
13 | Bilateral | R: <1 L: <1 | B33005 | R: C + 2ABC-, 1.5mA/60µs/130Hz L: C + 2ABC-, 1.8mA/60µs/130Hz | R: 1, 3 L: 1, 3 | R: 20.5 L: 19.5 |
A multivariate correlation analysis between subjective sleep scale scores, disease duration, DBS duration, and the scaled difference in LFP power between wakefulness and sleep found no significant correlations between any of the sleep scales and the scaled LFP metric. Further, there was no significant correlations between the temporal measures of disease and the scaled LFP metric. The only significant correlations were the following: disease duration and stimulation duration (r = 0.70, p < 0.05), PSQI and PDSS-2 (r = 0.80, p < 0.05), and FSS and PDSS-2 (r = 0.60, p < 0.05).