The ability to imitate develops from a very early age [1, 2]. Studies have reported that neonatal infants can imitate facial, oral, and hand gestures [3–5]. In subsequent development, imitation consolidates during unstructured dyadic interactions [2, 6, 7]. Over the first two years of life imitation behaviors become progressively more complex, starting from vocalizations and simple actions directed on objects (e.g., banging a toy) and extending to meaningful and meaningless manual and facial gestures [8]. The meaning and familiarity of gestures are parameters that influence the propensity and ability to imitate. Indeed, imitation of familiar, meaningful hand and facial gestures (e.g., applause, smiling) is mastered earlier than the imitation of meaningless gestures [2, 8].
The important role of imitation in development has been consistently recognized. Imitation appears as an essential tool for social learning and acquiring new skills during early childhood [2, 9]. Imitation promotes crucial developmental processes, including language [6, 10] and social skills [2, 8], as well as theory of mind [9] and joint attention [11]. Such early language and social abilities are core areas of differences associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [12–14], which is defined by difficulties in social communication and interaction and the presence of repetitive and restricted behaviors [15]. Given its implication in early social development, imitation has generated much interest in autism research [2, 8, 9].
Imitation research in ASD has mainly focused on two types of imitation, namely spontaneous imitation and elicited imitation performance [2]. Spontaneous imitation is assessed with naturalistic observations or parental reports. Most studies have reported lower frequency of spontaneous imitation in children with ASD (e.g., 16,17), whereas others have not (e.g., 18). The present paper focuses on elicited imitation, a paradigm in which individuals are explicitly instructed to imitate an action [2]. Previous studies have revealed disruptions in imitation performance in siblings at an increased likelihood for ASD. Focusing on the Autism Observation Scale for Infants (AOSI; 19), which includes an elicited imitation task, Zwaigenbaum et al. [20] highlighted that by 12 months of age, siblings of children with ASD who were later diagnosed with ASD showed lower imitation performance than those who were not diagnosed. Young and collaborators [8] used a 10-item imitation battery comprising manual and facial gestures and actions on objects in a longitudinal sample of siblings at an increased likelihood for ASD and TD infants aged 12 to 24 months. The authors showed that infants who later developed ASD exhibited delayed imitation development. We also recently showed that toddlers with ASD had difficulties in imitating both actions on objects and gestures [21].
Several explanations have been proposed to explain imitation difficulties in ASD. However, the mechanisms remain unclear. Vivanti and Hamilton [2] proposed that attentional, social, and executive factors may all play a key role in imitation in ASD. Attentional difficulties correspond to a lack of attention to the demonstrated actions, whereas social difficulties are involved in the processing and understanding of social information, and executive factors relate to difficulties in motor execution and performance of actions [22]. To examine these factors, Vivanti and collaborators developed innovative imitation tasks combining eye-tracking technology and video-recordings of children’s imitative behaviors [22–24]. In addition, Akin-Bulbul and Ozdemir [25] recently developed an eye-tracking paradigm combined with video-recordings, and studied six types of imitation (meaningful and meaningless gestures, vocalizations, and actions on objects) in toddlers with ASD. These studies [22–25] found contrasting results regarding both visual exploration and imitation performance. Vivanti and colleagues [23] found that children with ASD and without intellectual disability and their TD peers attended similarly to the action and to face regions during meaningless gestures, but the ASD group imitated less accurately. Similarly, Vivanti et al. [24] showed that although the number of fixations to the demonstrator’s face did not differ between ASD and TD groups, participants with ASD showed poorer imitation skills. Additionally, no significant correlation between imitation performance and visual exploration was found in either group. In a subsequent study using a different task, the authors [22] revealed that preschoolers with ASD spent less time looking at the demonstrator’s face and also showed poorer imitation skills than children with TD and developmental delay (DD). Akin-Bulbul and Ozdemir [25] showed that toddlers with ASD displayed diminished attention to the face and action regions. Moreover, these authors found that meaningful gestures were the only type of gesture where participants with DD showed better imitation performance than participants with ASD.
Several elements may be involved in these conflicting findings [22–25]. Firstly, these inconsistencies may result from heterogeneity in age and intellectual abilities across participants with ASD (i.e., from toddlers to adolescents with no intellectual disability). Secondly, the inclusion of different types of imitation (namely meaningful and meaningless actions on objects, manual and facial gestures, and vocalizations) may contribute to contrasting findings given that imitation with objects appears earlier and is easier for children with ASD compared to the imitation of gestures, which is more complex and may depend on familiarity [2, 26]. Indeed, research on imitation performance with older children, adolescents and adults with ASD tends to report differences that specifically affect meaningless gestures [2]. Additionally, Stone and colleagues [27] showed that while the imitation of body and facial gestures predicted speech development, imitation of actions with objects was associated with play, suggesting that imitation is not a unitary skill [7, 27]. It is established that imitation of gestures is more difficult than imitation of actions with objects in ASD [26, 28–30]. Nevertheless, to our knowledge, no study has yet examined meaningful and meaningless gesture imitation in a younger sample of children with ASD.
Thus, the present study aims to investigate the imitation of hand and facial gestures jointly with visual attention processes. Using an eye-tracking paradigm combined with video-recording, we compared three types of gestures, namely meaningful hand gestures, meaningless hand gestures, and meaningless facial gestures. To understand the processes behind imitation behavior better, we adopted an innovative approach and concentrated on factors relating to visual attention and motor execution in a sample of 84 children with ASD (aged 3.55 ± 1.11 years old) and 16 TD children (aged 3.31 ± 1.17 years old). We defined the visual attention factor as visual exploration toward the demonstration of gestures and to the actors’ social cues (i.e., when they asked the child to look at them and to imitate). Additionally, we explored the association between the children’s visual exploration and their imitative behavior. As to the motor execution factor, we investigated whether individual characteristics of children (e.g., age, developmental skills, autistic symptoms) were related to imitation performance. We hypothesized that young children with ASD exhibiting more attention towards the gesture demonstrations and the actors’ faces during social cues would display more accurate imitation. Furthermore, in line with previous literature [2, 20, 21, 25, 31], we hypothesized that children with ASD would show diminished attention to the demonstrators’ face and perform more poorly than their TD peers and that imitation performance would be positively associated with age and developmental skills. Such results would help to unravel the roles of visual attention and motor execution and their contributions to imitation behavior. Given the importance of imitation in early development, these results would further contribute to targeting and adapting existing interventions for ASD.