The primary aim of this study was to identify the spatial and non-spatial relationships between assigned landscape values and spatial perceptions of climate change risk in the Huon Valley coastalscape of Southern Tasmania to improve climate adaptation decision-making through reduced community conflict. In this pursuit, we answered three research questions, which we now address in turn. (1) Do social-psychological measurements of risk perception influence landscape values or spatial risk perceptions? In testing the influence of social-psychological correlates, precisely that of risk perception as defined by van der Linden (2015), there was limited evidence to indicate a strong relationship between the risk scales and mapping behavior. However, evidence suggests that landscape values are influenced by levels of risk perception, thus signaling the need to understand better the role social-psychological factors play in people’s conception of place and risk. (2) Are there statistical correlations between landscape values and spatial risk perceptions in the study area? Similar to other studies (Moser, 2014; Raymond and Brown, 2011), our analysis suggests a unique relationship between landscape values and spatial perceptions of risk in the case study region. In addition to the overall correlations between the landscape value typology and spatial risk categories, we found that relationships were particularly strong for specific value and risk combinations. These pairings sensibly align with the types of underlying features that would drive both the creation of value or attachment and exposure to risk – for example, recreational opportunities in wilderness areas at high risk of bushfires or values associated with recreating with family at foreshore areas at risk of sea level rise or flooding. (3) What are the spatial associations between all mapped values and risks and specific value and risk combinations? We determined that both the overall spatial associations and specific value/risk associations highlight key locations across the region that require both management priority and reframing of local council action and that the pairings provide both the spatial priorities as well as the types of actions needed to mitigate risks and manage values. While it is safe to assume that locations with combined high landscape value and high spatial risk perception will require increased management effort, their connections to specific values and risks offer both a challenge and an opportunity for councils to reframe their adaptation responses.
5.1 Influence of social-psychological correlates
Data analysis suggests that despite the high reliability of the social-psychological scales and their intercorrelations, their influence on assigned landscape value and spatial risk perception mapping behavior is not as strong or direct as hypothesized. Even the combined place attachment scale, which has previously demonstrated an influence on mapping behavior (Brown et al., 2015; Brown and Raymond, 2007; Williams and Vaske, 2003), yielded relatively few statistically significant outcomes in both spatial and non-spatial calculations. The other scales, specifically the adjusted ‘climate change risk perception scale’ and ‘future dissatisfaction scale,’ resulted in limited statistical significance. However, there were several notable correlations with mapping behavior. For example, an individual’s choice to map a particular number of climate change risk markers correlates with high risk perception levels and future dissatisfaction. Although for specific values and risks, the strength and direction were neither uniform nor predictable, thus indicating a complex relationship between the social-psychological correlates at the place-specific scale.
Our findings echo Raymond and Brown's (2011) recommendation of incorporating psychological factors better to understand drivers of risk for adaptation policy decisions. Researchers continue to demonstrate the complex relationships between perceptions of climate change risk and place attachment (Bernardo, 2013; Bonnes and Lee, 2017; De Dominicis et al., 2015; Etkin and Ho, 2007; Praskievicz, 2022), yet the divergences between different types of risk perception are not clearly reflected in the literature. For example, we attempted to measure and compare two types of risk perception: spatially assigned risk (e.g., specific locations perceived to be at risk due to climatic or other factors) and social-psychological determinants of risk or risk worldviews (e.g., internal thoughts and feelings about risk more broadly influenced by various social and nonsocial factors). While both seem to demonstrate a strong relationship with place attachment, as demonstrated in our research and other studies, their intersected influence is less easily described. Nevertheless, exploring the connections between the underlying influences on value and risk mapping provides a more meaningful understanding of why people place risks and further highlights the need to explore other social-psychological influences of mapping behavior.
5.2 Correlation between values and risk
Our data indicates that the decision for an individual to map specific values predicted the number and type of mapped risks in some cases. For example, an individual's likelihood to map several aesthetic values significantly predicted the number of mapped biodiversity changes, river flooding, and sea-level rise risk points. Across the data set, some values were good predictors of risk mapping behavior, whereas others were not significant. Individual and collective responses indicate that the importance and decision to map types of values and risks are related to factors beyond the type of mapped feature or the specific location being mapped.
Recreation was the most important value both in terms of total markers and mean importance but showed limited influence on the likelihood of an individual mapping risk. Furthermore, landscape values that demonstrated predictive capabilities for the number of mapped risk locations, such as Aesthetic or Biological, were not noticeably more likely to share high levels of spatial association with the paired risk types. These findings closely align with previous studies (Raymond and Brown, 2011). Further complicating these results is that the values and risks demonstrated a high degree of Complete Spatial Randomness while tending to show the strongest spatial and non-spatial relationships with other variables. While this may partially be a product of smaller sample sizes for marker types or the structure of the case study region, it also highlights the complex correlations between values and risks.
Across the different statistical analyses, the most relevant values (as determined by mean importance, spatial randomness, and spatial/non-spatial correlations) were ultimately Recreation, Aesthetic, and Wilderness. These results reflect how people describe their reasoning for relocating to the region (Denny and Pisanu, 2019; Osbaldiston, 2022). For risks, the most relevant were Bushfire, Sea-level rise, and Biodiversity loss. These risks are typically described as being the most prescient for Tasmanians (Lyth et al., 2016), with bushfires being the most likely risk to have previously experienced (Buys et al., 2012), while both sea-level rise and biodiversity loss are commonly discussed as risks in Tasmania (Grose et al., 2010). In most cases, these relevant values and risks had medium to strong intercorrelations, excluding Wilderness and Sea-level rise, where the former was marked as an exclusively terrestrial landscape feature. Linking climate adaptation actions for the three main risks to the relevant landscape value type aligns with values-based approaches widely seen as critical to increasing community acceptance of local climate change actions (Adger et al., 2013; Artelle et al., 2018; Persson et al., 2015). Individual and compound climate risks manifest in places and features distinctively and thus impact specific values in different ways (Hess et al., 2008). Our results indicate that people are uniquely suited for identifying links between specific risks and assets, primarily due to their knowledge about the places they value and which threats are likely to be present. Decision makers must consider the unique combinations of assigned values, perceived risks, and landscape assets, as each requires discrete adaptation actions.
5.3 Spatial associations of select risks and values
While there was a high degree of variability in the mapping behavior of participants, spatial patterns emerged in all analyses of landscape values and spatial risks. Values and risks are not randomly distributed across the region, including those demonstrating some degree of spatial randomness. In fact, they tend to cluster around on-ground assets such as towns, protected areas, or other essential features. For example, as shown in Fig. 4, areas with high value and high spatial risk perception are most prominently around the townships along the Huon River, Hartz Mountain, and areas around the Southwestern National Park. When it comes to decision-making around climate change at the local level, many of these areas would already be of high interest to planning authorities due to existing population concentrations or identification as critical tourist infrastructure, as would be the case for Hartz and Tahune. This is a significant area for concern as many of these locations have recently been determined to be most suitable for future development (Huon Valley Council, 2023), while limited adaptation planning remains in place. It suggests that the council needs to develop a more robust rationale for steering development into at-risk places or quarantining risky places from future development (Braunschweiger and Ingold, 2023).
The spatial distribution of both Recreation and Aesthetic values tends to be assigned to areas under the local council's authority. In contrast, Wilderness appears in areas beyond their authority to act, meaning that jurisdiction is under state or national authority. When implementing a values-led approach to adaptation (Artelle et al., 2018), local councils must focus on the values most aligned with their authority to act and the specific action type. For example, the Huonville Foreshore, a highly valued local area at risk of sea-level rise and flooding, is under local management compared to the Egg Island Conservation Area, an equally highly valued area with similar risk, which is under state control. Both elicit strong sentiments from community members for protection but receive differing levels and forms of adaptation from the council. In some cases, councils may need to pressure higher levels of government to ensure local landscape values are duly conserved (Wewerinke-Singh and Salili, 2020).
For areas with specific combinations of relevant high values and risks (including Bushfire and Sea-level rise), there are several variables and issues for planning authorities to consider, which relate directly to conceptualizations of climate change risk more broadly (Buys et al., 2012). For example, the council struggles with mitigation bias (Measham et al., 2011). Politically, council factions are driving mitigation responses (e.g., using electric vehicles) despite their limited contribution to national and global emissions. However, adaptation to select risks or key locations is lacking. Like many local governments and rural areas, the council will be forced to grapple with the decision to defend in place or undertake managed retreat (Abel et al., 2011), regardless of any other prior climate actions. The response to bushfire risk in recreation value areas will likely require a combination of bushfire preparedness (Frandsen et al., 2011), managing risk (Neale et al., 2019), and reframing the relationship between Bushfire, landscape, and community (Kruger and Beilin, 2014; Reid et al., 2020). The response to risks in wilderness areas typically falls under a combination of preservation and restoration (Mendel and Kirkpatrick, 2002; Wilson et al., 2007). However, as is the case in Huon Valley, these areas are often beyond the scope of the authority of local councils, or the efforts are mismatched with the threat level (Mendel and Kirkpatrick, 2002; Rands et al., 2010).
5.4 Implications for adaptation planning
We have identified several challenges for adaptation planning at local levels. We concur with Raymond and Brown (2011) that government agencies, particularly at the local level, have limited resources and thus must target priority areas with shared high value and high risk. By assessing and reflecting on specific landscape-level assets and unique combinations of values and risks in relation to those assets, our approach builds on their methodology to provide more precise and policy-relevant recommendations. As such, it would not be controversial to suggest that the Huon Valley Council targets the priority areas described in the preceding sections, especially since these locations seem to both fall under the council's purview and have established resource needs.
For example, our data indicates that people perceive their recreational values as most at risk from bushfires or sea-level rise. Recreational values tend to cluster around towns and community centers. While bushfires and sea-level rise overlap in select locations with recreational values, their presence is a significant concern across the region. For decision-making, without considering how risks and values anchor to specific physical assets, there is a possibility of expending resources to protect Recreational value areas or to reduce the risk of either bushfires or the consequences of sea-level rise (i.e., coastal inundation). Since the Recreational values tend to cluster with several other values, their perception of risk and loss to the community could be overestimated. Additionally, the consequences of landscape value loss are not proportional to the actual loss from climate change impacts, possibly forcing the need to prioritize specific value types over others.
Furthermore, there are significant differences between individual spatial perceptions of risk, the discursive conception of risk, and the projected on-ground risks, with the latter being the primary source of information for decision-making. This study's primary area of focus was the overlap between spatial risk perceptions and landscape values and limited attention to the effect of climate risk discourses on spatial perceptions of risk. People’s climate risk discourses are not exclusively based on a comprehensive understanding of climate change impacts, as indicated in the risk perception literature (Adger et al., 2013; McDonald et al., 2015; O’Neill and Nicholson-Cole, 2009; Rawluk et al., 2019; van der Linden, 2015). Since we cannot assume that spatial perceptions of risk reflect projected risk but are a byproduct of their risk discourses, and decision-makers rarely utilize community understandings of risk in policymaking, a gap must be addressed to reduce conflict and increase understanding of climate actions.
For instance, bushfires were the most widely placed climate change risk, even when accounting for outliers. Nevertheless, the risk markers tended to concentrate closer to values and along the Huon River, which, while certainly at risk of Bushfire, is significantly less so than bush areas that make up most of the region. The higher perception of bushfire risk may be more experientially related to recent bushfire events either in the region or across Australia, cognitively related through the widespread focus of Bushfire as a significant risk both presently and increasingly into the future throughout Australia, or socio-culturally related to people’s feelings around the potential impact to themselves and their more-than-human community (Booth and Harwood, 2016; Lange and Gillespie, 2023; Neale, 2018).
5.5 Future research opportunities
Both the extent of climate risk discourse influence on spatial risk perceptions and the gap between spatial risk perceptions and projected climate risk are topics suitable for future research. While identified as having limited evidence in this study of the role climate risk discourses play in mediating an individual’s mapping preferences, we believe that greater emphasis should be placed on understanding the social-psychological correlates play at the place-specific scale. Researchers have found that PPGIS can explore these correlates when used as part of a suite of methods (Elwood, 2010), including group valuation (Nijnik and Miller, 2017), visioning exercises (Ungar et al., 2020)d method (Author(s), XXXX). Additional social-psychological methods would be essential in uncovering the underlying discourses of concern (Buys et al., 2012; Etkin and Ho, 2007; Fløttum and Gjerstad, 2017). Similarly, exploring the various landscape value discourses would help support values-led management and planning (Albizua and Zografos, 2014; Rozema and Bond, 2015; Zografos and Martínez-Alier, 2009). Specifically, a multidisciplinary approach focused on social-psychological correlates can improve the framing of adaptation actions, thus bolstering a social license to act (Cradock-Henry et al., 2018).
Also beyond the scope of this paper was a comparative analysis between people’s spatial perception of risk and the projected climate risks (Brown et al., 2004). Since the local council has exclusively relied on projected risks for management and planning decisions, identifying areas where there is a high perception of risk and low projected risk, and vice versa, could assist local governments in reducing conflict between stakeholders and decision-makers (Carmen et al., 2022; Cunningham et al., 2023). Additionally, this paper discussed the role of the land management authority, which can limit the potential scope or ambition of local adaptation actions due to ambiguity or contestation over jurisdiction. Through a more comprehensive demonstration of the associations between management authority, landscape values, and risk perceptions, local councils could better communicate the challenges of adapting to climate change within existing policy frameworks (Hanssen et al., 2013; Measham et al., 2011).