3.1 Verification of emotional IBS
In this section, we verified whether emotional IBS exists in audio-visual stimulus-sharing scenes and tested the regulatory role of emotion parameters (Valence and Arousal) in emotional IBS.
3.1.1 Emotional IBS in the dyad
We regrouped the DEAP into 40 videos according to the ID labels of the videos. Each packet contained EEG data collected from 32 participants while watching the same video and the corresponding emotion parameters. This packet is called a video group (VG).
First, we calculated the overall PLV for each VG. After selecting every possible binary combination in the 32 segments of EEG data,496 pairs of combinations were formed in each VG. For each combination, we calculated the PLV values between the two EEG sequences collected from the electrodes at the corresponding positions of the different participants.
We then averaged all PLV values over the combinations and called this PLV value the total average PLV used as the baseline for emotional synchronization comparison. The total average PLVs for all VGs were obtained separately at five EEG characteristic frequencies (Delta:0.5-3 Hz, Theta:4-7 Hz, Alpha:8-13 Hz, Beta:13-30 Hz, and Gamma:31-50 Hz). Second, according to the ‘Valence’ (one of the emotion parameters), 32 segments of EEG data in a VG were divided into three parts (negative, neutral, and positive). The average PLV values of the three groups were calculated. This calculation method is consistent with that for the total average PLV calculation method, except for the data grouping operation. Third, according to ‘Arousal’ (another emotional parameter), the 32 segments of EEG data in a VG were divided into three groups (passive, neutral, and active). The average PLV values of the three groups were obtained similarly to the total average PLV.
The calculation results are listed in Table 1. Under the ‘Valence’ condition, the PLV values of ‘Neutral’ were greater than the total average PLV in the Delta, Theta, and Alpha frequency bands. The rest included a ‘Negative’ emotion in Alpha and a ‘Positive’ emotion in Gama. Participants having a ‘Positive’ emotion were prone to emotional IBS only in the Gama frequency band. Under the ‘Arousal’ condition, the PLV values under the ‘Passive’ condition were greater than the total average PLV in nearly all characteristic frequencies except Beta. Participants in the ‘Neutral’ and ‘Active’ emotion categories did not produce emotional IBS easily, except in the Beta and Gama frequency bands.
We performed a one-sample t-test to identify the frequency of significant Emotional IBS (p < 0.05). Under the ‘Valence’ and ‘Arousal’ conditions, variance analysis was applied to PLVs of 40 VGs (40×6×5) and Total Average PLV. As shown in Table 2, under the ‘Valence’ condition, there was a significant difference between Total Average PLV and ‘Neutral’ emotion PLVs in the alpha frequency band. In the beta frequency band, there was a significant difference between ‘Negative’ emotion PLVs and total average PLV. Under the ‘Arousal’ condition, the PLVs in ‘Passive’ emotion were significantly different in the alpha frequency band. In the theta frequency band, PLVs of ‘Active’ emotion were significantly different.
We synthesized the results of PLV baseline comparison and analysis of variance and extracted the frequencies adhering to the requirements of PLV greater than the Total Average PLV and significant difference (p < 0.05). Participants with ‘Excited’ and ‘Calm’ emotions were most likely to produce emotional IBS (Red Squares in Figure 1). The corresponding characteristic frequencies were the theta and alpha values. Second, there was an obvious emotional IBS (Erythema Squares in Figure. 1) in the 'Depressed,' ‘Relaxed,' and 'Neutral' emotions; that is, the PLV in the 'Valence' and 'Arousal' dimensions were greater than total average PLV at the same time, but it did not show a common significant difference. It was concluded that emotional IBS most likely occurs in the alpha band.
3.1.2 Relationship between valence/arousal differencesand PLVs
For the 40 VGs, each group consisted of 496 participant dyads. Differences in the Valence’ and ‘Arousal of each dyad were extracted. For each VG in the five frequency bands, the PLV values (averaged over all electrode-dyads) were reordered in ascending order according to the differences of ‘Valence’ and ‘Arousal.’ We averaged the sorted PLV sequence and its corresponding ‘Valence’/‘Arousal’ differences over 40 VGs. Thus far, we have obtained a more macroscopic distribution relationship between the PLVs and differences in emotional parameters. No clear linear relationship was observed between them (Figure 2).
We fused all the data trials in the DEAP. The labels of the video and participants were removed, and only the EEG data and emotional parameters were preserved. The total data included 1280 segments of EEG data. We constructed 816003 data dyads. Differences in the Valence’ and ‘Arousal of each dyad were extracted. Thus, a one-to-one relationship between PLVs and differences in emotional parameters at the five frequencies was established. Statistical analyses were applied to emotional IBS. Two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) of the frequency bands (delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma) and emotional parameters (Valence and Arousal) were conducted.
As shown in Table 3, the differences in emotional parameters had no significant effect on the distribution of PLV values (p>0.05). The PLVs could not indicate how the emotional IBS was influenced by the ‘Valence’/‘Arousal’ differences.
3.2 Multilateral hyperscanning
We propose a hyperscanning method based on brain connectivity analysis and an Affine Invariant Riemannian Metric (AIRM) on a Symmetric Positive Definite (SPD) manifold. In this section, we verified whether a hyperscanning algorithm based on this concept is suitable for multilateral interactions. Notably, PCA was performed to reduce the dimensions of the brain connectivity matrix. Based on this, we obtained the principal component characteristics in the same dimensions extracted from the brain connectivity matrices. Then, AIRM Metric was used to represent the similarity between the individuals’ emotional states.
3.2.1 Emotional IBS in the group
One VG contained 32 segments of EEG data. After obtaining the PCA features of the brain network matrices, 496 pairs of PCA feature dyads with the same dimensions were formed in each VG. We calculated the SPD values in five frequency bands within each dyad and averaged them over the 496 PCA feature dyads. This mean PLV value, called the total average SPD, was used as the baseline for emotional synchronization comparison. According to the ‘Valence,’ 32 segments of EEG data in a VG were divided into three groups (negative, neutral, and positive). The average SPD values of the three groups were calculated. The calculation method for each group was consistent with that for the Total Average SPD. Then, according to the ‘Arousal,’ 32 EEG data segments in a VG were divided into three groups (passive, neutral, and active). The average SPD values of the three groups were calculated.
As shown in Table 4, under the ‘Valence’ condition, the SPD values of ‘Neutral’ were smaller than the total average SPD in the delta, theta, and alpha frequency bands. The rest included a ‘Negative’ emotion in Delta and a ‘Positive’ emotion in Gama. Under the ‘Arousal’ condition, the SPD values of ‘Passive’ were smaller than the total average PLV in all characteristic frequencies. The rest included a ‘Negative’ emotion in beta. The results of this part were basically consistent with section 3.1.1.
3.2.2 Relationship between valence/arousal differences and SPD metrics
As described in Section 3.1.2, there were 496 participant dyads in each VG. Differences in the Valence’ and ‘Arousal of each dyad were extracted. In each VG, in the five frequency bands, the SPD values were ordered in ascending order according to the differences in ‘Valence’ and ‘Arousal’. The sorted SPD sequence and its corresponding ‘Valence’/‘Arousal’ differences were averaged over 40 VGs. Therefore, the distribution relationships between SPD values and emotional parameter differences were obtained, as shown in Figure 3.
As shown in Figure 3 (a), in the range of small differences (<1), the SPD has an inverse linear relationship with the differences in valence. In the large difference range (3-8), SPD had a positive linear relationship with the ‘Valence’ differences. In the intermediate difference range (1-3), there was no linear relationship between the SPD and ‘Valence’ differences. As shown in Figure 2 (b), in the small difference range (<1), SPD had an inverse linear relationship with ‘Arousal’ differences. In the large difference range (5-8), SPD had a stronger inverse linear relationship with ‘Arousal’ differences. In the intermediate difference area (1-3), there was no linear relationship between SPD and ‘Arousal differences.
Statistical analyses were performed to verify the significance of emotional IBS. Two-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) of the frequency bands (delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma) and emotional parameters (Valence and Arousal) were conducted. As shown in Table 5, the differences in ‘Valence’ significantly affected the distribution of SPD values in frequency theta, alpha, and beta (P<0.05). The differences in ‘Arousal’ significantly affected the distribution of SPD values only in the theta frequency (P<0.05). This result proved that the proposed hyperscanning method better indicated how the emotional IBS was influenced by the ‘Valence’/‘Arousal’ differences. And the emotion parameter ‘Valence’ was the more dominant factor in emotional IBS.
3.3 Variation characteristics of emotional IBS in time dimension
In this section, we verified whether emotional IBS weakens over time with gradual familiarity with the content of audio-visual materials. The structure of the SEED dataset was relatively simple. Each participant watched 15 videos with different emotional contents in a single trial. The experiment was repeated three times on three different days. Following the introduction of the SEED, each participant performed the experiment once with an interval of approximately one week. Each trial had a total of 45 files with the extension: mats (MATLAB files).
The EEG data from all trials in SEED shared the same label sequence. The EEG data labels were unified for each trial. This raised a question. The emotional responses produced by different participants watching the same films were consistent. The emotional states produced by participants watching the same group of videos three times were the same. Owing to this series of questions, this study did not analyze from the perspective of the emotional labels of the datasets. By ignoring the labels of the participants and films, we studied the variation characteristics of emotional IBS over time.
In each trial, the EEG data collected from 62 electrodes showed artifacts in varying degrees. Therefore, the data must be preprocessed before format deformation. The preprocessing included low-SNR electrode removal and artifact data segment removal. The processed data contained different numbers of electrodes and sampling points. Therefore, heterogeneous EEG data were obtained. For each participant, the experiment was repeated thrice on three different days. The EEG data contributed by the participants on the first, second, and third days were divided into three data groups called the date group (DG), which included 225 segments of EEG data. For one segment of EEG data, the brain connectivity matrices at five characteristic frequencies were first calculated. The dimensions of the brain connectivity matrix depended on the dimensions of the processed data (electrode × sample point). Therefore, there may be dimensional differences in the data contributed by different participants, even for the same participant in different trials. This further led to dimensional differences in brain connectivity matrices. After the principal components were extracted, all brain connectivity matrices were transformed into unified dimensions, the 225 PCA features were organized into dyad combinations, and SPD measures were obtained for each dyad. We analyzed the overall SPD value distribution characteristics over three days.
In Figure 4, the rows represent the five characteristic frequencies. Columns represent time dimensions. The x- and y-axes of each subgraph represent the data dimension because the processed data were heterogeneous, and the data dimension deviated. The z-axis of the subgraph marks the SPD between the brain-connected matrices. The figure clearly shows that the emotional differences (SPD values) between participants decreased with time at all five frequencies, and the emotional responses of the participant groups to the same video stimulus materials gradually became similar. This pattern was slightly weak in the Gama frequency band but decreased daily, as shown in Table 6.