Estimates suggest that 296 million people use drugs, which represents an increase of 23% over ten years. 39.5 million individuals suffering from a substance use disorder (SUD), this represents an increase of 45% over ten years annually (UNODC, 2017). Twenty-two million people have problems with cocaine, and these numbers are disproportionately represented by cocaine consumption among countries in Central and South America. Within this region, the annual prevalence of cocaine use is highest among Chile (1.1%), Argentina, Uruguay (1.6%), and Costa Rica (1.2%) (UNODC, 2017).
In addition to the burden of disease, substance use has important implications for broader indicators of well-being. While 9.3% of the general population in Brazil has experienced some form of violence, exposure to violence is more than twice that (19.7%) among cocaine users (Abdalla et al., 2018). The likelihood of being involved in violent crimes increases by a factor of 4 among cocaine users (Abdalla et al., 2018). While causal links cannot be ascertained from these statistics, it is worrying that individuals who use cocaine are at greater risk of being the victims and perpetrators of violent crimes. Given the evidence that certain drugs lead to Disinhibition (De Wit, 2009; Garavan & Hester, 2007), cocaine use may be directly linked to violent crime (Markowitz, 2005; Parker & Auerhahn, 1998). However, it is also possible that socio-economic status accounts for the link between substance use and violence (Vermeiren et al., 2003).
The same factors underpinning exposure to violence and drug abuse among those of low socioeconomic status (SES) may also be related to other deficits in daily life functioning. People with substance use disorder (SUD) are known to be linked to deficits in socio-affective and cognitive function (Ersche et al., 2011; Potvin et al., 2014; Preller, Hulka et al., 2014; Verdejo-Garcia, 2014). These deficits may be the precipitant to or consequence of regular drug use. The effects of frequent drug use, such as blunted reward and general Disinhibition, may render specific individuals more likely to experiment with certain drugs. Such vulnerability is expected to be more pronounced among individuals from lower socio-economic backgrounds (Redonnet et al., 2012). Lack of rewarding social encounters, along with insufficient social support, may impact initial and continued drug use (Daniel et al., 2009; Galea & Vlahov, 2002). Lack of social support and adaptive coping strategies, along with easy access to drugs, is likely to promote and maintain use among individuals from lower socio-economic strata (Little & Steinberg, 2006). Continued usage is expected to dynamically interact with vulnerability factors, creating further social and functional impairments.
Intentional harm is a critical component of effective social interaction, which implies understanding the goals, intentions, and mindset of others (i.e., Theory of Mind) (Decety et al., 2012). Misunderstanding the choices of others is likely to lead to miscommunication, arguments, and unpleasant interpersonal experiences. Specifically, intention recognition or inference is critical to Empathy and moral decision-making processes (Frith & Frith, 2012; Van Overwalle, 2009). Intention recognition refers primarily to the ability to cognitively determine if an action performed by another person is accidental or voluntary (Baez et al., 2016; Escobar et al., 2014a). Intentional harm detection involves early and late brain responses involving the amygdala and frontotemporal coupling (Hesse et al., 2016). Intentional harm detection requires integrating complex processes such as emotion and cognition and is moderated by individual psychological traits (Blakemore & Decety, 2001; Castiello, 2003; Heberlein, 2008). Intention recognition has been studied in the context of moral decision research (Decety & Cacioppo, 2012a), the development of social abilities in young people (Escobar et al., 2014b), and dementia (Baez et al., 2016). Despite impaired socio-cognitive function among stimulant drug users (Quednow, 2017; Volkow et al., 2011) and the potentially critical role that lack of intentional harm detection may play in facilitating interpersonal deficits and heightened violence exposure among drug-affected individuals, no one has investigated intentional harm detection in a sample of cocaine abusers. More importantly, such work has not been conducted while controlling SES (an essential potential confound).
The Intentional Inference Task (IIT) (Escobar et al., 2014a) assesses fast intention inference regarding actions involving harm to others (intentional vs. unintentional) with different targets (Object vs. Person) (See Fig. 1). The IIT task primarily studied moral behavior and its brain correlates in healthy people (Decety & Cacioppo, 2012b). Also, it has been used to study the effects of early social deprivation on the intention inference capabilities and the extent of deficits due to frontotemporal dementia and frontal lesions (Baez, Couto et al., 2014). While the IIT is typically used to study moral decisions, it is also ideally designed to differentiate how individuals perceive the perpetration of violent behavior against objects and individuals, intentionally and unintentionally (Decety et al., 2009). Given the links mentioned above between substance use (cocaine use in particular) and violent behavior, as well as known deficits in socio-cognitive processes among individuals with SUD (Quednow, 2017; Volkow et al., 2011), we employed the IIT to explore the potential influence of chronic cocaine use, as well as numerous psychological factors (i.e., empathy, sensation-seeking, executive function) on intention inference. Alterations to the brain's early and late temporal dynamics have been observed among event-related potentials (ERPs) during this task (Decety & Cacioppo, 2012b). Different amplitude modulations have been apparent for intentional vs unintentional conditions, likely reflecting functions such as emotion processing, arousal activation, and cognitive evaluation. The most pronounced effects of this task have been reported in frontal electrodes (Escobar et al., 2014a).
For this study, we aimed to examine the neural signatures (i.e., event-related potential - ERP) linked to behavior during the IIT. Given the number of factors influencing socio-cognitive processes, especially among individuals with varied socio-economic backgrounds, it is critical to control for potential confounds. In the present study, Cocaine polydrug users (COC) were compared to a demographically and socioeconomically matched healthy control group (HC). To ascertain possible influences of early childhood upbringing, we assessed individual differences in parental bonding (See supplementary table 1). We also collected measures of Empathy, sensation-seeking, and executive function; these variables may constitute confounds and indirect ways individuals with SUD exhibit socio-cognitive impairments.
Given that attention is affected in periods of abstinence and related to behavioral performance (Bolla et al., 2000; Colzato et al., 2009), Click or tap here to enter text. We predicted that COC would be slower and less accurate than HC in the intentional inference task. Also, we predicted that COC would exhibit worse performance than HC for executive functions, consistent with impairments in general executive function (Hagen et al., 2016). We also predicted group differences in Empathy and sensation seeking (COCs exhibiting less Empathy and more sense-seeking than HCs). At the neural level, we expected a difference in HC ERP modulation relative to COC in pre-defined frontal regions of interest (ROI) (Escobar et al., 2014b). Specifically, we predicted that COC would exhibit less amplitude modulation than HC, corresponding to less emotional arousal (Imbir et al., 2015). We expect group differences to be significantly pronounced for intentional vs unintentional condition comparisons.