Our study provides a first description of gestural sequence use in a wild group of neotropical primate species, Geoffroy’s spider monkeys. Our results largely confirmed our hypotheses. As in apes, gestural sequences were common in spider monkeys and appeared to be triggered by emotional arousal. In particular, gestural sequences were mostly produced in play contexts (in line with Prediction 1) and often continued even after triggering the recipient’s response (in line with Prediction 2). Moreover, gestural sequences were mostly produced by males (in line with Prediction 3) and younger individuals (in line with Prediction 4), mostly toward younger recipients (in line with Prediction 5).
This study provides the first evidence of gestural sequence use in species other than apes. Spider monkeys strongly relied on gestural sequences to communicate. The majority of gestures were incorporated into sequences, which mostly included 2–4 gestures, and up to 18 gestures. However, sequences did not appear to be complex combinations used to convey different meanings to recipients. In line with Prediction 1, and with literature in apes (Genty and Byrne 2010; Liebal et al. 2004; Tanner and Perlman 2017; Tempelmann and Liebal 2012), most gestural sequences were produced in a play context. In particular, the proportion of gestures produced as part of a sequence (rather than as single gestures) was highest in the contexts of play, aggression and sexual behaviour, which are likely to imply higher arousal than contexts like travelling, resting, grooming, feeding or foraging. Also in line with Prediction 2 and with literature on apes (Tempelmann and Liebal 2012), gestural sequences persisted regardless of whether recipients responded, suggesting that they were likely triggered by emotional arousal.
Out of the 125 sequences observed during this study, only three (i.e., embrace-pectoral sniff, push-present climb, grab-grab pull) occurred more than twice and were not mere repetitions of the same gesture type. This suggests that these three specific sequences might be recurrent in the gestural communication system of spider monkeys. In literature, embraces (i.e., monkeys wrap one or two arms around the recipient’s back or shoulder, while facing each other) and pectoral sniffs (i.e., monkeys orient their face and nose toward the recipient’s chest-axilla; Schaffner and Aureli 2005; Villa-Larenas et al. in press) are indeed described as species-specific affiliative gestures that often occur concurrently (Rondinelli and Lewis 1976; Schaffner and Aureli 2005). Both embraces and pectoral sniffs are thought to facilitate the regulation of social relationships in spider monkeys, by reducing the risk of aggression in contexts that are likely to be associated with tension (Schaffner and Aureli 2005; Schaffner et al. 2012), like fusion events and infant handling (Aureli and Schaffner 2007; Rondinelli and Lewis 1976; Schaffner and Aureli 2005; Slater et al. 2009). By implying physical contact and exposing vulnerable body parts to other group members, however, also embraces and pectoral sniffs can entail some risk (Schaffner and Aureli 2005). Therefore, their co-occurrence may be adaptive to increase the likelihood of clearly conveying the sender’s affiliative intention and to prevent aggressive escalations in potentially dangerous contexts.
With regards to push-present climb, there is no literature we are aware of on spider monkeys, but it seems plausible that these gestures co-occur because they may serve a complimentary function, to attract the recipient’s attention before facilitating climbing onto the signaller’s body. Finally, grab and grab pull are defined in a very similar way in literature, as the signaller holding the hand firmly closed over the recipient’s body, but also exerting some force to move the recipient from his position in case of grab pull (e.g., in spider monkeys: Villa-Larenas et al. in press; in chimpanzees: Hobaiter and Byrne 2011). There are therefore at least two reasons why grab and grab pull may often co-occur. First, they might serve a very similar function and thus often be used concurrently in the same situation. Second, top-down classifications might have wrongly considered grab and grab pull as two different gesture types, whereas they might be simple variations of the same gesture type, so that their co-occurrence is rather a repetition than a sequence of different gesture types. To address this issue, it will be essential in the future to use more objective bottom-up approaches to identify gesture types in this species (see Mielke et al. 2023).
Across individuals, the probability of producing gestural sequences varied in line with our predictions. First, in line with Prediction 3, males were more likely than females to produce gestural sequences. These findings are in line with literature on apes. In chimpanzees, for instance, long gestural sequences were only produced by males (Liebal et al. 2004), and sequences were more likely produced in dyads that included at least one male, rather than only females (Amici and Liebal 2022). Male chimpanzees have also been reported to show high levels of persistence and produce long gestural sequences in consort to recalcitrant females – a context which is likely to imply high arousal as compared to other contexts (Hobaiter and Byrne 2011b). Notably, sex differences in the production of gestures in spider monkey appear to be limited to gestural sequences, rather than to single gestures (Villa-Larenas et al. in press). Second, in line with Prediction 4, younger signallers (i.e., infants and juveniles) were more likely than older ones (i.e., subadults and adults) to produce sequences. These results are in line with previous studies in apes (Amici and Liebal 2022; Hobaiter and Byrne 2011b; Liebal et al. 2004), and confirm that gestural sequences are mostly used by inexperienced individuals and become less likely through age, as communication becomes more effective through interactional experience and exposure to others’ gestures (Amici and Liebal 2022; Fröhlich et al. 2018; Hobaiter and Byrne 2011b). Finally, in line with Prediction 5, individuals were more likely to produce gestural sequences toward younger recipients (i.e., infants and juveniles) than toward older ones (i.e., subadults and adults). These results suggest that signallers are more likely to produce sequences when interacting with less experienced recipients, who may be less responsive or less effective in their communication and thus increase signallers’ arousal.
Our study faced several limitations. First, it was based on relatively few observations (i.e., 709 gestures and 125 sequences), so that additional observational effort might allow detecting other recurrent sequences with different properties. However, some studies in great apes have also relied on a comparable number of sequences (e.g., Liebal et al. 2004). In the same line, our study only included one group of monkeys, failing to capture possible intra-specific variation. Second, our study relied on a top-down categorization of gesture types and sequences, which might have failed to faithfully capture the meaning and function of single gestures. Although they have been seldom used in literature (Bard et al., 2019; Mielke et al. 2023), finer-grained classifications based on bottom-up approaches might allow a better identification of gesture types and sequences, and provide different results (Amici et al. 2022). Third, the study of gestural sequences would surely benefit from a multimodal approach, as it cannot be excluded that monkeys rather rely on multicomponent and multimodal combinations of signals to convey novel meanings (Amici et al. 2022). Future studies should address this gap by evaluating whether and how spider monkeys combine gestures, vocalizations and/or facial expressions, and how recipients respond to these sequences. Fourth, it would be interesting to also incorporate physiological measures to assess emotional arousal when individuals produce gestures and gestural sequences. In the wild, thermos-cameras represent a promising non-invasive tool for such investigations (Nieuwburg et al. 2021; Travain et al. 2021). Finally, it would be important to complement our study with observations in captivity, as visibility in the wild might sometimes be limited for arboreal species, and this may have compromised visibility during observations in some contexts (e.g. while travelling).
Overall, our results showed that sequences are recurrent in the gestural communication of wild spider monkeys and that they are likely the result of increased emotional arousal, resembling patterns observed in great apes. These findings provide a first contribution to the study of gestural sequences in monkeys, and confirm neotropical primate species as a valid complimentary model to the study of primate gestural communication.