Two experiments were performed to test whether the decomposition of a complex motor skill facilitates the learning of that skill. In total, 60 skilled and 12 non-skilled pianists participated in the study.
Experiment 1
The purpose of experiment 1 was to test whether the skill decomposition in practicing, together with the visualization of subtle movement errors, facilitates the learning of a complex motor skill that is extremely difficult to perform even by pianists who have undergone years of extensive piano training since childhood. Thirty-six skilled and 12 non-skilled pianists participated in the experiment, which consisted of a pretest, intervention, and posttest sessions (Fig.1A).
Experimental task
The experimental task required the alternate repetition of two movement patterns of simultaneous strikes of two keys; one the right index and ring fingers (movement pattern A) and another with simultaneous keystrokes with the right middle and little fingers (movement pattern B) (Fig.1B-a). This task is obtained from Etude Op. 25 no. 6 composed by Frédéric Chopin, being known as one of the most technically demanding pieces. Each trial consists of successful repetition of one cycle of two movement patterns 20 times. If keypresses were performed in an incorrect order (e.g., pressing the keys with the index and little fingers instead of the middle and little fingers for the movement pattern B), those keypresses were not counted in the successful 20 repetitions, and that trial was considered an error trial. We asked the participants to repeat the experimental task 10 times in the pretest and posttest sessions with maintaining a predetermined fastest tempo for each individual, which was determined for each participant as one with the probability of the error trials being approximately 6 out of 10 trials. As an index of the motor performance, we counted the number of the error trials among the 10 trials (i.e., keypress error).
Training
To confirm the ceiling effect of the conventional training on the complex motor skill, we conducted the repetition training of the experimental task. Twelve skilled pianists were instructed to play as quickly as possible while minimizing the probability of the error occurrence [9 women, 22.3 ± 2.7 years old]. The training of the repetition of the experimental task consisted of 30 trials, each of which had 20 strikes of the keys. They took a 3-minute break every 5 trials.
Another 24 skilled pianists were randomly divided into two groups with different interventions: the feedback (FB) group [skilled FB group: n = 12, 1 man, 22.9 ± 2.3 years old (mean ± SD)] and the no-FB group [n = 12, 5 men, 21.8 ± 2.1 years old]. All non-skilled pianists were allocated to the FB group [non-skilled FB group, n = 12, 2 men, 23.7 ± 5.6 years old]. All participants individually practiced two movement patterns, which were constituent elements of the experimental task (Fig.1B-b). One involved the simultaneous strikes of two piano keys leaving one white key in between with the index and ring fingers while releasing the two depressed keys adjacent to the keys to be struck with the middle and little fingers at the same time (i.e., movement pattern A). Another (movement pattern B) is the inversion of the movement pattern A (i.e. keystrokes with the middle and little fingers and key-releases with the index and ring fingers). In the training session, participants in the FB and no-FB groups were instructed to execute each movement pattern discretely, with synchronizing the timing of the movements across four fingers (Fig.1A-b). Following the execution, the participants in the FB group received visual feedback regarding the timing of each finger movement (i.e. temporal synchrony of movements between the fingers). The visual feedback information included the onset of keypress by each of the four fingers within a time window of 0-100 ms. In addition, if the difference in the timing between the earliest and latest finger movements (i.e., timing error) is lower than 10 ms or larger than 20 ms, circles corresponding to each finger displayed in the monitor turned black or blue, respectively; otherwise, it turned red. Thus, these three colors roughly indicated the amount of error. The participants were instructed to minimize the timing error based on the visual FB throughout the training session. The participants in the no-FB group were instructed to minimize the timing error without provision of the visual FB sorely based on the tactile information at the keypresses. The training of each movement pattern consisted of 10 blocks, each of which had 30 trials. The participants took a 5-minute break every 5 blocks. Half of the participants practiced the movement pattern A first and then practiced the pattern B, whereas another half practiced the movement patterns in the opposite order.
Before and after the training session (i.e., pretest and posttest sessions), all participants (i.e., participants of repetition and decomposition trainings) underwent the experimental task to assess the change in the task performance.
Performance of the experimental task
Figure 2A illustrates the mean values of the keypress error across the skilled pianists who underwent the repetition training of the experimental task in each of the pretest and posttest sessions. To confirm the repetition training effect on the motor performance, we used a generalized linear mixed effect model (GLME) with a Poisson distribution because it was a count data (fixed effect: session; random effect: participant). The GLME did not yield a significant difference in the keypress error between the pretest and posttest sessions (χ2 = 3.1, P = 0.08).
Figure 2B illustrates the group means of the keypress error in each of the pretest and posttest sessions for the skilled FB, no-FB, and non-skilled FB groups. The results confirmed that the keypress error in the pretest session did not differ across all groups. This is because the movement tempo was personalized so that the probability of error trial could become 60% for each participant. To assess the decomposition training effect on the motor performance, we used a GLME with a Poisson distribution (fixed effects: group, session, and their interaction; random effect: participant). The GLME identified a significant interaction effect between the factors (χ2 = 6.8, P = 0.03). A simple effect test unveiled a significant reduction in the keypress error specifically in the skilled FB group (z ratio = -2.7, P < 0.01). Moreover, we calculated a mean value of the interval between repeated keystrokes of the two movement patterns in each trial of the experimental task, in order to verify whether the movement tempo changes between the pretest and posttest sessions. We used a GLME with a logarithm link function and a gamma distribution (fixed and random effects were same as the keypress error) due to non-negative values and the distribution skewing towards 0. The GLME showed no significant difference in the movement tempo between the sessions (χ2 = 1.5, P = 0.21).
In summary, the skilled FB group showed an improvement in probability of the error occurrence while maintaining the movement tempo. On the contrary, the repetition training of the experimental task and the decomposition training in each of the skilled pianists without error feedback and in the non-skilled pianists did not show such improvement.
Timing error
Figures 2C (a) and (b) illustrate the time course of the timing error derived from the training session for the movement patterns A and B in the skilled FB, no-FB, and non-skilled FB groups. To test whether the timing error was changed through training based on the difference in the timing error at the block 1, we used a GLME with a logarithm link function and a gamma distribution (fixed effects: group, block, movement pattern, and their interactions; covariate: timing error at block 1; random effects: participant, participant×block, participant×movement pattern). A GLME yielded main effects of group (χ2 = 10.2, P < 0.01) and timing error at the block 1 (χ2 = 137.9, P < 0.01), and an interaction effect between group and block factors (χ2 = 35.5, P < 0.01) on the timing error. A post hoc test revealed significant group differences in the timing error at the block 5 (skilled FB < non-skilled FB: z ratio = -3.2, P < 0.01), at the block 6 (skilled FB < non-skilled FB: z ratio = -2.6, P = 0.02), at the block 7 (skilled FB < no-FB: z ratio = -3.0, P < 0.01; skilled FB < non-skilled FB: z ratio = -3.4, P < 0.01), at the block 9 (skilled FB < no-FB: z ratio = -3.1, P < 0.01; skilled FB < non-skilled FB: z ratio = -3.8, P < 0.01), and at the block 10 (skilled FB < no-FB: z ratio = -3.3, P < 0.01; skilled FB < non-skilled FB: z ratio = -3.9, P < 0.01). This indicates the timing error was reduced through the training of both the movement patterns specifically in the skilled FB group.
Figures 2D (a) and (b) illustrate the group means of the timing error derived from the pretest and posttest sessions for the movement patterns A and B in the skilled FB, no-FB, and non-skilled FB groups. To confirm the decomposition training effect on the timing error during the experimental task, we used a GLME with a logarithm link function and a gamma distribution (fixed effects: group, session, movement pattern, and their interactions; random effects: participant, participant × session, participant × movement pattern). The GLME yielded a significant main effect of the movement pattern (χ2 = 6.0, P = 0.01).
In summary, for the skilled FB group, the result of the training showed improvement of the timing error in both movement patterns A and B, whereas the training effect on the timing error did not reach it during the experimental task.
Experiment 2
Experiment 1 demonstrated that the decomposition of complex motor skill with provision of the visual FB on the error information in training improved the timing error for both movement patterns during training session and the keypress error of the complex skill in the skilled pianists. By contrast, the result of the experimental task showed no improvement in the timing error for both movement patterns. The lack of improvement may be attributed to the number of trials, although this aspect remains unclear. Moreover, we found the reduction of the keypress error by the decomposition training, whereas the underlying mechanism remains unclear. To clarify these, in Experiment 2, we asked participants to intensively practice movement pattern B, which turned out to be more challenging than movement pattern A (because the timing error of the experimental task was larger in the movement pattern B than in the movement pattern A) and examined the effects of training on the kinematics of multi-finger movements. Twenty-four skilled pianists participated in Experiment 2, where they were trained solely on movement pattern B while recording the angle of the metacarpophalangeal (MCP) and proximal interphalangeal (PIP) joints of 4 fingers using a data glove implementing sensors recording the joint angles (Fig. 1A).
Experimental task
Some participants in the skilled FB group in Experiment 1 showed no error in the task performance after the training. To prevent this floor effect, the participants in Experiment 2 performed the experimental task with a movement tempo that was confirmed to elicit approximately 8 error trials out of 10 trials at preliminary investigation done prior to the pretest session, indicating an increased task difficulty Experiment 2 compared to Experiment 1.
Training
Participants were randomly divided into two groups with different interventions: the FB group [n = 12, 4 men, 26.6 ± 5.3 years old] and the no-FB group [n = 12, 1 man, 22.9 ± 4.2 years old]. Based on the results that facilitation of the exploration of the movement pattern B correlated with improvement in the motor performance, the participants in both the FB and no-FB groups were instructed to perform the movement pattern B with and without the visual FB regarding the timing of the finger movements in the same manner as Experiment 1. Both the FB and no-FB groups underwent a training session consisting of 600 trials (30 trials × 20 blocks) and took a 5-minute break every 5 blocks. In addition to the measurement of the piano keystrokes, we also measured the MCP and PIP joint angles of the 4 fingers using the data glove during the pretest, posttest, and training sessions.
Performance of the experimental task.
Figure 3A illustrates the group means of the keypress error in the pretest and posttest sessions for both groups. This value in the pretest session was higher in Experiment 2 than Experiment 1, reflecting the increased task difficulty in Experiment 2. A GLME with a Poisson distribution for the keypress error (fixed effects: group, session, and their interaction; random effect: participant) yielded significant main (group: χ2 = 6.1, P = 0.01, session: χ2 = 10.4, P < 0.01) and interaction (group×session: χ2 = 6.0, P = 0.01) effects. Post hoc tests identified significant improvement of the keypress error through the training specifically in the FB group (z ratio = -3.9, P < 0.01) but not in the no-FB group (z ratio = -1.0, P = 0.35).
Figures 3B and C illustrate the group means of the timing error for the movement patterns A and B in the pretest and posttest sessions for both groups. A GLME with a logarithm link function and a gamma distribution (fixed effects: group, session, movement pattern, and their interactions; random effects: participant, participant×session, participant×movement pattern) revealed a significant interaction effect between session and movement pattern factors (χ2 = 10.6, P < 0.01). Post hoc tests found a significant session-wise difference in the timing error for the movement pattern B (z ratio= -2.3, P = 0.02), but not the movement pattern A (z ratio = -0.4, P = 0.72).
Changes in the finger joint movements
Because the decomposition training simplifies intricate movements so that they can be executed and perceived, it is postulated that the exploration of the finger movements is facilitated during the learning process. To test this hypothesis, we assessed changes in the finger joint movements throughout the training. Figures 3D and E illustrate averaged time-varying finger joint angles across 600 trials in the training session at one representative pianist of each group. Positive and negative values indicate extension and flexion of the finger joints, respectively.A shaded area of the time course of the finger joint angles, which represents the variance across trials, was larger for a pianist of the FB group than for one of the no-FB group.
To quantify the amount of the exploration during the training session, we first computed the angular velocities (Figs. 3F and G: same pianist as in Figs. 3D and E, respectively) and then computed the amount of change in the angular velocities between two consecutive training blocks. Specifically, we first calculated the average peak angular velocity of each finger joint across trials for each training block. Thus, one vector consisting of angular velocity values of the eight finger joints was computed for each block (i.e., 20 vectors in total). Then, we computed the Euclidean distances in the angular velocity vector between all possible pairs of two adjacent training blocks (i.e., 19 pairs). This reflects the time course of the change in finger joint movements between adjacent blocks. We refer this value as a variability index (VI). A high VI value indicates different patterns of the finger joint movements between the two blocks (i.e., large exploration), whereas a low VI value indicates performing similar finger joint movements between the two blocks (i.e., low exploration).
Figure 3H illustrates the group mean of the VI in the FB and no-FB groups. A linear mixed effects model (LME: fixed effects: group, block, and their interactions; random effects: participant) revealed significant main (group: χ2(1) = 4.0, P = 0.04; block: χ2(18) = 69.29, P < 0.01) and interaction effects between the factors (χ2(18) = 59.1, P < 0.04). Post hoc tests yielded group differences in the VI at each of the block 1 (t(344) = 3.4, P < 0.01), at the block 2 (t(338) = 3.0, P < 0.01), at the block 7 (t(338) = 3.4, P < 0.01), at the block 8 (t(338) = 2.0, P = 0.03), at the block 9 (t(338) = 3.0, P < 0.01), and at the block 10 (t(338) = 3.0, P < 0.01).
A relation between finger joint coordination pattern and motor performance
To identify whether the amount of the exploration of the finger joint movements is associated with the improvement of the performance of the experimental task by the training, we computed Pearson’s correlation between the VI and the change in the keypress error between the pretest and posttest sessions. Overall, the VI was larger in the first few blocks than the subsequent blocks in the FB group, and was kept constant in the last few blocks in both groups. We thus used the difference in the VI between the blocks 1-2 and 19-20 for this analysis. Figure 3I illustrates the scatterplots of the differential value of the VI relative to the differential value of the keypress error in each of the FB and no-FB groups. We found a significant positive relationship between these variables specifically in the FB group (R=0.66, P=0.02) but not in the no-FB group (R=0.25, P=0.44).