All study participants' demographic characteristics and prognostic variables, and mean ages of participants classified into different GMFCS, MACS, and CFCS levels were shown in Table I. Children at each of the three levels of the classification systems demonstrated similarity in the age (p>0,05). Among the 98 enrolled children, 57 (% 58,2) were in good functioning levels in MACS (level I-II), whereas 41 (% 41,8) were in moderate functioning level (MACS level III). On average, % 55,1 could walk independently in all environments (GMFCS I), % 38.1 could walk with some limitations (GMFCS II), and only % 6.1 required a hand-held device for mobility. Considering that the MACS is a classification system of disabled children's hand function in five levels (a lower value describes higher manual ability), most of the children in this study had effective hand use (MACS I-II). For communication skills, the predominant levels were level I (% 56.1) and level II (% 29.6) respectively, while fewer participants had a moderate score (level III; %14.3). That is, most of the study participants could independently and effectively communicate with people in most environments. Finally, except for hearing impairment, vision, speech, and cognition impairment was reported in 9 (% 9.2), 11 (%11,2), and 12 (% 12,2) of study participants, respectively.
The distribution of children at different MACS levels within GMFCS levels was demonstrated in Table II. As outlined, most children in MACS level I (89,3%) were qualified with GMFCS I; on the other hand, a very few percent (10,7%; 3 children) presented GMFCS II. Among children in MACS II, 55,2% was at GMFCS I; likewise, 44,8% was at GMFCS II. Finally, more than half children in GMFCS III (53,7%) featured level III of GMFCS.
Table III demonstrates comparison of participants’ executive function tasks (manual ability) and participation in different life situations scaled scores on ABILHAND-Kids and CASP questionnaires according to MACS levels. Analysis of one-way ANOVA showed that the executive function and participation in different life situations outcomes exhibited variability in participants at each of the three MACS levels (p= 0,000). In other words, children in high functioning levels in MACS gained greater both executive functions and participation scaled scores on ABILHAND-Kids and CASP questionnaires than children of moderate functioning level (Post-Hoc: a>b>c). Moreover, Figure 1 and 4 illustrated the range of scaled scores on ABILHAND-Kids and CASP questionnaires with % 95 CI. According to Figure 1, children in MACS I demonstrated greater variability as to manual performance than children in MACS II and III.
Children in level I, II, and III at GMFCS differed greatly for executive function tasks and participation in different life situations outcomes (p=0,000) (Table IV). Furthermore, results of pairwise post-hoc tests suggested that participants in a higher functioning GMFCS level had more significant outcomes in both executive function tasks and participation in different life situations than participants with a moderate functioning level (Post-Hoc: a>b>c). That is, the higher GMFCS levels were found to be associated with greater mean scores on both the ABILHAND-kids and CASP questionnaires. Children in GMFCS I presented greater variability for executive function tasks (ABILHAND-Kids) than those in GMFCS II and III. In contrast, children in GMFCS III demonstrated greater variability in scaled scores on CASP-Community Participation subdomains than those in GMFCS I and II (Figure 2 and 5)
Table V shows executive function tasks and participation in different life situation outcomes by communication skills on CFCS instrument. Results of one-way ANOVA displayed statistical meaningful differences among three levels related to both manual ability and participation in different life situation outcomes (p=0,000). However, isolated comparisons (pairwise post-hoc tests with Tukey) revealed a statistically significant difference only between high and moderate levels (CFCS I-III), whereas it was not found out statistically meaningful differences between level I and II, and between level II and III (Post-Hoc: a>c, a=b, b=c). In other words, only children at level I and level III in CFCS exhibited variability in executive function tasks, and participation scaled scores on ABILHAND-Kids and CASP questionnaires. In contrast, a significant difference could not be found between children classified into CFCS levels closer to each other for actual performance in daily life. Furthermore, as demonstrated in Figure 3 and 6, children qualified with CFCS III displayed greater variability in scaled scores on both executive function tasks and participation in different life situations than those in CFCS I and II.
As demonstrated in Table VI, MACS levels were found to be moderately correlated with GMFCS levels (r=0,491, p=0,000), whereas, weakly correlated with the CFCS levels (r=0.247, p=0,014). Finally, a moderate relationship was observed between GMFCS and CFCS levels (r=0,574, p=000).