In the present study, we demonstrated that shouting significantly increased the handgrip force level of MVC, followed by an increase in pupil size and a reduction of the cortical silent period. Such an enhancing effect of shouting on handgrip MVC is generally consistent with results of previous studies (1, 2). Our findings indicate that the pupil-linked neuromodulatory system and the motor system are more excitable as a result of shouting, which leads to the production of additional muscular force in maximal exertion efforts. These results indicate that maximum volition-induced motor system activity does not drive muscles to produce the full force of which they are capable; that is, there is a latent ability for producing additional force that is hidden in ordinary force exertion.
Pupil size as a fine index of actual intensity of the handgrip contraction
Pupillometry has long been used as a measure of brain state. A number of studies
have reported that pupillary dilation is related to mental effort (cognitive load), and the correspondence between cognitive load and pupillary dilation has been documented in several contexts, including paired-associate learning (23). The pupil size is well known to increase according to the complexity of the mental task (23). A recent study has demonstrated that the pupil size also increases during physical effort, the degree of which reflects the actual intensity of muscular contraction (24). Thus, we examined pupil size during handgrip MVC between the shout and control conditions. Because we consider that the effects of noradrenaline (NE) on the activity of motor cortical neurons as described below is indirectly estimated by pupil size, which is also supported by some previous studies (25-27).
Enhancing effect of shouting on motor system activity through the potentiation of the pupil-linked neuromodulatory system
Shouting significantly increased pupil size and reduced the cortical silent period. Changes in pupil diameter are considered to correspond to the activity of neuromodulators, including NE and acetylcholine, which produce alterations in the brain state and corresponding changes in behavior. It is currently unknown whether activity in only noradrenergic locus coeruleus (LC) neurons directly influences pupil size; however, noradrenergic neurons are active during pupillary dilation (28).
The effects of NE on the activity of motor cortical neurons are presumed to be as follows. First, NE increases the excitability of large pyramidal cells in layer V of the rat motor cortex (29). Second, NE depresses evoked inhibitory postsynaptic potentials in the rat sensorimotor cortex through the presynaptic inhibition of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)ergic interneurons (30, 31). Third, in pyramidal cells in layer V of the rat prefrontal cortex, NE increases the frequency of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic potentials (32-34). These enhancing effects of NE on the activity of motor cortical neurons result in increased intracortical facilitation and reduced intracortical inhibition. Such enhancing effects of NE on the activity of motor cortical neurons results in a reduced duration of the cortical silent period, as was observed in the current study (Fig. 4A). Although our experimental design did not allow the direct assessment of intracortical inhibition, changes in silent periods of longer than 100 ms, as recorded in the hand muscles of healthy participants (35), are considered an index of cortical inhibition (19). The site of origin of the cortical silent period is largely in the M1 (19), in which GABABergic circuits may generate the cortical silent period (36–38). Thus, shouting is likely to reduce inhibition in the M1, which enhances motor excitability.
Despite the aforementioned enhancing effect of NE on motor cortical activity, we failed to detect any changes in MEP amplitude during MVC; there were no significant differences in MEP amplitudes between the shout and control conditions (Fig. 4B). This failure to detect any change might be associated with recruitment of the M1 neurons to reach a plateau level during MVC. In other words, most of the M1 neurons had already been recruited (39); thus, fewer neurons were available to respond to TMS. Thus, the M1 neurons recruitment to reach a plateau level during MVC might have overshadowed any differences in MEP amplitudes between the shouting and control conditions.
Relationship between shouting and pupillary dilation
The production of ‘shouting’ necessitates two pathways, which are organized hierarchically, building from the basic levels in the lower brain stem and spinal cord to the most
complex levels in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the laryngeal motor cortex (LMC), respectively (40). The limbic vocal control pathways responsible for the control of innate nonverbal and emotional vocalizations, and the laryngeal motor cortical pathway modulates the fine motor control of voluntary voice production—such as speech and song—as well as the voluntary production of innate vocalizations. Coordination and interactions between the LMC and ACC–periaqueductal gray (PAG) pathways are indispensable for proper voice control and voice initiation in the shout condition. In the present study, participants in the shout condition were asked to shout and perform handgrip MVC (see Participants and Procedures). The LMC requires inputs from the inferior frontal gyrus for motor planning of voice production, and from the supplementary motor area to prepare for vocal motor command execution. In the limbic vocal control pathway, the PAG mainly plays a gating role in triggering a vocal response and regulating its intensity, whereas the ACC is involved in the voluntary control of voice initiation and its emotional intonation. Thus, coordination and interactions between the LMC and ACC–PAG pathways are invaluable for the self-generating shout.
The LMC does not only connect reciprocally with motor cortices, such as the surrounding ventral and dorsal premotor cortices, primary motor cortices, and supplementary motor area, but also with subcortical regions, such as the claustrum, ventral and mediodorsal thalamus, medial parabrachial nucleus, deep mesencephalic nucleus, and LC (40). The PAG receives direct projections from the ACC as well as from other cortical and subcortical regions that control the limbic, sensory, motor, cognitive, and arousal systems (41). The PAG receives noradrenergic projections from the brain stem reticular formations (e.g., the LC, ventrolateral medulla [A1], and dorsal pons [5th]). Thus, shouting may stimulate the activity of LC neurons, possibly by enhancing the activity of the LMC and ACC–PAG pathways, which results in pupillary dilation.
Pupillary dilation was not observed during the MVC task phase, but also during the experimental instruction phase. The cause of pupillary dilation during the experimental instruction phase is assumed to be as follows. First, pupillary dilation during this phase may be caused by motor imagery, performing the handgrip MVC combined with self-generated shout immediately after the instruction was given. Indeed, when debriefed, participants reported that they imagined the combined motor action. Some previous studies have reported such a motor imagery-induced pupillary dilation (42–44). Although each motor action (i.e., handgrip or shout) is essentially primitive, the combined motor action execution requires the processing of higher-order motor control (see the previous descriptions of central shouting control). Thus, motor imagery during the instruction phase may induce pupillary dilation. Another cause may be related to the time pressure associated with the motor imagery: the participants had to complete the rehearsal activity before the text stating “1st time” was unpredictably displayed on the monitor, instructing the participants to start squeezing the handgrip device immediately after the experimental instruction. Some previous studies reported that such time pressure is inherent in the structure of a mental task and induces particularly large pupillary dilations (42, 45). Thus, time pressure associated with motor imagery may have induced pupillary dilation in the current study.
Consequently, we cannot exclude the possibility that pupil-dilating effects during the experimental instruction phase might influence pupillary dilation during the handgrip MVC phase with shouting. However, the percentage increase in pupillary dilation during MVC (12.1% ± 2.7%) was much greater than that during experimental instruction (8.5% ± 2.9%). Thus, a change in pupillary dilation during the experimental instruction phase cannot adequately explain the difference in pupillary dilation across tasks. We therefore consider that the pupillary dilations in the experimental instruction and MVC task phases had different main causes.