To understand the participants’ characteristics better, we first describe their sensory processing abilities, EF, and cognitive deficits in daily activities, as measured by the AASP, BRIEF, and DLQ, respectively.
Sensory Processing
As presented in Table 2, most participants had normal sensory-processing abilities and were found to be in the normal range of each sensory profile of the AASP.
[Insert Table 2 about here]
Executive dysfunctions
Table 3 depicts the sample’s EF as measured by the BRIEF-A. The mean scores describe normal values. When measuring the prevalence of executive dysfunctions, most (77%) participants had normal EF (i.e., scored less than 65), whereas 23% had behavioral dysregulation/executive dysfunctions (scored 65 or more).
[Insert Table 3 about here]
Daily activities performance
Table 4 depicts participants’ cognitive abilities expressed in IADL performance as measured by the DLQ. Scores ranged from 1 (no difficulty) to 3 (much difficulty).
[Insert Table 4 about here]
Differences in EF between people with and without altered sensory processing
According to Aim 1, we examined differences in EF (BRIEF-A) and cognitive deficits in IADL performance (DLQ) between participants with and without altered sensory processing. Those with altered sensory processing showed reduced EF, as measured by most BRIEF scales, than did those in normal ranges of the AASP quadrants. As shown in the mean differences, people with lower ability to register sensory input scored significantly lower in inhibition (M = 7.69, p = .001), shifting efficiency (M = 8.18, p = .004), working memory (M = 9.31, p = .007), behavioral regulation (M = 9.93, p = .001), and GEC (M = 7.53, p = .01) than did those with normal registration.
No significant differences in BRIEF-A scores were found between people in the sensory-seeking profile ranges. However, as described here by mean differences, participants with extreme sensitivity to sensory input had significantly lower scores in inhibition (M = 6.78, p < .0001), initiation (M = 6.68, p = .003), working memory (M = 9.77, p < .0001), plan/organize (M = 9.46, p < .0001), task monitor (M = 6.02, p = .03), behavioral regulation (M = 7.35, p < .0001), meta-cognition (M = 8.69, p < .0001), and GEC (M = 8.19, p < .0001) than did those in the normal range of sensory sensitivity.
People with extreme sensory avoidance scored significantly lower, as shown by mean differences, in inhibition (M = 7.64, p = .001), emotional control (M = 8.50, p = .02), initiation (M = 9.11, p < .0001), working memory (M = 12.05, p < .0001), plan/organize (M = 14.37, p < .0001), task monitor (M = 7.51, p = .02), behavioral regulation (M = 8.55, p = .005), meta-cognition (M = 10.37, p < .0001), and GEC (M = 10.11, p < .0001) than did those in the normal range of sensation avoidance.
Figure 1 depicts the visual presentations of the mean scores of each BRIEF-A scale in each sensory processing pattern.
Differences in daily activities performance between people with and without altered sensory processing
Participants with altered sensory processing showed greater deficits in IADL performance (as measured by the DLQ) than did those scoring in the normal ranges of the AASP quadrants:
participants with lower ability to register sensory input had more difficulties to perform activities related to household (mean difference = .24, p = .006). Participants with greater sensory sensitivity had more difficulties to perform activities as reflected by the following DLQ scales: language (M = .33, p< .0001), community/participation (M = .22, p = .02), complex tasks (M = .28, p = .01), multitask organization (M = .34, p = .002), memory (M = .31, p < .0001), monitoring (M = .35, p < .0001), activity participation (M = .23, p = 001), and cognitive symptoms (M = .33, p < .0001) compared to those in those found in the normal sensory sensitivity range.
Participants with extreme sensation avoidance also had greater to perform activities as reflected by the following DLQ scales: language (M = .23, p = .04), memory (M = .25, p = .02), monitor (M = .32, p < .0001), and had greater cognitive symptoms (M = .25, p = .02) than those found in the normal sensory avoidance range (see Figure 2).
Relationships between age, EF, sensory processing and daily activities performance
Based on the literature that emphasizes the relationships between aging, worse sensory processing, executive dysfunction and restricted ability to perform daily activities, together with the results described above, a SEM model was created. This SEM model aimed to provide detailed information about the relations between age, the four sensory processing patterns presented in Dunn’s model, the EF as measured by the BRIEF, and examine whether EF mediate between sensory processing and daily activity performance
The SEM model revealed good fit indices, including normed fit index (NFI), χ2(15) = 21.68, p = .12; CFI = .98; NFI = .97; RMSEA = .05. There was a direct effect between age and sensory seeking (β = -.20, p = .009), in which older participants had less tendency to seek sensory input in their daily environment. The effect between age and low registration was close to significant (β = .13, p = .08), as was the effect between age and sensory avoiding (β = .14, p = .06). Hence, these results show a tendency for the older participants to have higher avoidance and greater difficulty registering sensory input. Nevertheless, low registration and sensory sensitivity were directly related to BRIEF-A scores (β = .32, p < .001; β = .40, p < .001 respectively). This means that greater difficulties with register sensory input correlated with worse EF (BRIEF-A) and with greater cognitive deficits expressed in IADL performance (DLQ). Further, the BRIEF-A was directly related to the DLQ total score (β = .39, p < .001), but no significant indirect effect was found between age and BRIEF-A scores or between age and DLQ scores.
Sensory sensitivity and low registration had significant indirect effect to the DLQ (.16 and .12, respectively) mediated by the BRIEF-A for sensitivity (p = .001) and low registration (p = .002), 95% CIs [.07, .31], [.04, .27], respectively (see Figure 2). This model explains 46% of the BRIEF-A variance and 42% of the DLQ variance.
In sum, older people had less tendency to seek sensory input in their daily environment. Their greater difficulties registering sensory input correlated with worse EF (BRIEF-A) and greater cognitive deficits expressed in IADL (DLQ). Sensory sensitivity and low registration had significant indirect effects with DLQ mediated by BRIEF-A (Figure 3). Hence, executive dysfunctions may worsen the negative effects of altered sensory processing on daily activity performance in older adults.
[Figure 1-3 about here]