Recruited Population
N= 414 Italian children, aged 8 to 10 years old, attending 4th or 5th grade of public primary school in Lombardy, Northern Italy, were recruited. Referrals were obtained from professionals taking part in an ongoing research and placement training programmes. A first screening, administered by teachers, based on the Italian versions of the ADHD Rating Scale for Teachers (SDAI)[10][39] and the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale (DBDRS)[38] were used to assess ADHD traits in the participants prior the beginning of the study.
Inclusion Criteria
Inclusion criteria for all participants were as follows: (1) had to be between 8 and 10 years of age, (2) have a score above 70 in the verbal and performance IQ on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - IV Edition (WISC-IV), and (3) have no history of brain damage, epilepsy, psychosis, autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorders (early-onset bipolar disorder), Tourette’s syndrome, childhood depression, oppositional defiant disorder; (4) have no current aggressive behaviour or severe oppositional tendency; (5) have no hearing, visual, or physical disabilities, and (6) not being under psychiatric medication.
Furthermore, all participants had to show good understanding of the concepts of time and money. The Concepts of Time and Money Questionnaire (CTMQ) was developed for this study and is reported in the supplementary materials. The cut-off was set to 15 correct answers among 16 questions in both time and money.
Participants included in the EG also have a cut-off severity scores of 14 or higher in both ADHD-I (inattentive subgroup) and ADHD-H (hyperactive subgroups) subscales, i.e. ADHD-C criteria (combined inattentive and hyperactive subgroups) on SDAI and a clinical diagnosis of ADHD based on DSM-5[3] criteria obtained from a licensed clinical child psychiatrist. The diagnosis was further confirmed through an additional assessment by the consensus of experienced clinicians in the research team (i.e. child psychiatrists and clinical psychologists).
Measures
The SDAI is an ADHD questionnaire widely used in Italy, validated and standardized for the Italian population [11][46]. It is composed by eighteen items matching the symptom domain of ADHD as described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) [3]. It has a reliability of 0.80 (Inattentive subscale) and 0.74 (Hyperactive/Impulsive), optimal discriminatory power and concurrent validity (r > 0.95;[39]. Its test-–retest reliability is respectively 0.83 and 0.81 for Inattentive and Hyperactive/Impulsive [37]. The DBDRS is a 45-question screening measure, completed by either parents or teachers, designed to identify symptoms of ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder in children and adolescents.
Participants
Teachers were asked to observe the recruited children for two weeks and to complete SDAI and DBDRS for each child. Subsequently, they had to report the frequency of any problematic behaviour according to a Likert scale from 0 (problematic behaviour never presents) to 3 scores (very often present) for each questionnaire.
Thirty children were eligible for the EG according to the ADHD-C criteria on SDAI and were further assessed. Subsequently, the research team administered to the eligible children the CTMQ. Each participant was asked to answer 16 open-ended questions on knowledge regarding the values of money and time. Based on the results, N=22 children were eligible for the EG.
WISC-IV [52], DBDRS and the Parent Interview for children Symptoms (PICS-IV) scales were administered. These scales aimed to identify disruptive behaviours disorders or other psychiatric disorders, together with getting information about school achievement from parents, teachers, and students themselves. After this test, N=6 children were excluded because diagnosed with oppositional defiant disorders (figure 1).
The selected children were individually examined by experienced professionals of the research team and parents were interviewed to confirm their authorisation to be part of the EG.
------------------- Insert Figure 1 here ----------------
N=16 children (11M:5F; mean age in years = 8.75; SD = .48) fully met the inclusion criteria and were enrolled as EG in the present study. A randomized block selection among other children eligible for the CG was used to ensure that CG was balanced for number of children and gender. N=16 children (11M:5F; mean age in years = 8.62; SD = .62) were randomly assigned to the CG. Demographic data for both experimental and control groups are summarised in Table 1.
The EG displayed significantly higher scores than CG on both SDAI hyperactivity, t(22.4) = 38.5, p < .001, SDAI inattention subscales, t(15.0) = 70.5, p < .001, while there were no significant statistical differences in IQ, t(30) = 1.29, p = .208.
------------------- Insert Table 1 here ----------------
Experimental Design
Each of the N=32 participant was asked to engage in an individual assessment where the dual-task paradigm was administered. The digit span of each participant was assessed before starting the individual assessment using the digit span forward in the WISC-IV [52]. Assessments’ data are reported in the Supplementary Materials, sheet S1.
The first task consisted on a series of choices between a small monetary reward obtained after a shorter time and a larger monetary reward obtained after a longer time. The delayed reward option task comprised a series of 16 trials. In each trial, the participant was asked to select between two options e.g. the first option was always a smaller amount of money the same day, whilst the second option was a larger amount of money deferred in different periods of time. Each participant knew that there was no right or wrong choices. Trials comprises 4 possible rewards combinations: €1-€11, €2-€12, €3-€13 and €4-€14, with a reward difference always set to €10; and 4 possible time delays of 1 week, 1 month, 6 months and 1 year. The full set of results from the money/time option task is reported in the Supplementary Materials, sheet S2. An algorithm specifically designed for the test had select the order of the 16 trials randomly and balanced them among participants.
The second task was the memory load task. It consisted in memorising and repeating a series of random numbers using the same structure of the digit span forward in the WISC-IV [52]. In the digits forward task, the participant is required to listen and repeat the assigned sequence of numbers. Starting from a total of 3 digits, the number of digits to repeat would increase if the participants successfully manages to repeat all of them. A new number sequence was randomly generated before each trial.
If an error in repeating the digits is made, another series of the same digits is presented; if the participant fails also to repeat the second series, the task is stopped and the number of correct digits is recorded as the maximum digit span of the participant for that specific trial (see Supplementary Materials, sheet S2).
A dedicated application was developed for the administration of the individual sessions. All sessions were administered in a quiet classroom of the school, equipped with computers, monitors, chairs and desks. Each session lasted 20 minutes and all participants were asked to sit in front of a monitor in order to complete the tasks. Three different conditions were tested in the same order for each participant: (1) delayed reward option task without the memory load task; (2) dual-task with delayed reward option task and the memory load task administered together, with half memory load and (3) dual task with full memory load.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analysis was run through SPSS software (v. 23, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). The descriptive statistics of the dependent variables were examined. The dependent variable (the value of k) were submitted to repeated measure ANOVA with one between-subject factor (group), and two within-subject factors: (1) three memory load conditions (zero load, half load, full load) and (2) four time delays (1 week, 1 month, 6 months and 1 year); trials within the same condition but different monetary rewards were averaged together. The alpha-level was set to .05 for all statistical tests. All effect size and post-hoc tests’ power are reported in sheet S3 of Supplementary Materials. The effect sizes were computed using partial eta-squared. Omnibus tests were evaluated with two-tailed alpha-level = .05. Planned pairwise comparisons were performed among groups using t-test and ANOVA, alpha-level was Sidak’s corrected. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used for effects failing the Mauchly’s test of sphericity.
The initial full load was set to the maximum digit span forward minus one (n-1) that was preliminary assessed, while the half load was set to n/2 if n was even and (n-1)/2 if n was odd.
With reference to the primary task parameter, the inverse formula of was calculated: . k is a parameter that measures the decrease rapidity in subjective value over time. A k value of zero shows a certain preference for delayed choice, while higher k values show an increase in DD. In our analysis we will use euros for A and V and weeks for D (setting a month to 4 weeks) as units. The specific units of measurement that have been chosen change the result only by a multiplicative factor, the choice is therefore irrelevant for the purposes of statistical analysis.
Preliminary ANOVA was carried out to examine the secondary task performance, assuming digit span as the dependent variable to ensure that the relevant variable was the memory load and not the memory span.
Ethics, consent and permissions
The study was conducted in accordance with the American Psychological Association’s (APA) ethical standards. The study design was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of Cognitive Science, Psychological, Educational and Cultural Studies of the University of Messina approved the study protocol (Protocol n. 20015_34). All the parents of the children who took part in the study signed a written consent form.